Journal of Modern Foreign Psychology
2022. Vol. 11, no. 3, 140–151
doi:10.17759/jmfp.2022110313
ISSN: 2304-4977 (online)
Spatial conceptual mapping of words with temporal semantics
Abstract
General Information
Keywords: embodied cognition, temporal words, STEARC, language comprehension, spatial conceptual mapping
Journal rubric: General Psychology
Article type: review article
DOI: https://doi.org/10.17759/jmfp.2022110313
Funding. The reported study was funded by RFBR, project number 19-313-51023.
For citation: Malyshevskaya A.S., Gallо F., Pokhoday M.Y., Kotrelev P.V., Shtyrov Y.Y., Myachykov A.V. Spatial conceptual mapping of words with temporal semantics [Elektronnyi resurs]. Sovremennaia zarubezhnaia psikhologiia = Journal of Modern Foreign Psychology, 2022. Vol. 11, no. 3, pp. 140–151. DOI: 10.17759/jmfp.2022110313. (In Russ., аbstr. in Engl.)
References
- Fumarola A., Prpic V., Luccio R., Umiltà C. A SNARC-like effect for music notation: The role of expertise and musical instrument. Acta Psychologica, 2020. Vol. 208, article ID 103120, 8 p. DOI:10.1016/j.actpsy.2020.103120
- Bender A., Beller S. Mapping spatial frames of reference onto time: A review of theoretical accounts and empirical findings. Cognition, 2014. Vol. 132, no. 3, pp. 342—382. DOI:10.1016/j.cognition.2014.03.016
- Bergen B. K., Chan Lau T. T. Writing direction affects how people map space onto time. Frontiers in Psychology, 2012. Vol. 3, article ID 109, 5 p. DOI:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00109
- Bi S., Perkins A., Sprott D. The effect of start/end temporal landmarks on consumers’ visual attention and judgments. International Journal of Research in Marketing, 2021. Vol. 38, no. 1, pp. 136—154. DOI:10.1016/j.ijresmar.2020.04.007
- Battelli L., Cavanagh P., Martini P., Barton J. J. S. Bilateral deficits of transient visual attention in right parietal patients. Brain, 2003. Vol. 126, no. 10, pp. 2164—2174. DOI:10.1093/brain/awg221
- Boroditsky L. Language and the Construction of Time through Space. Trends in Neurosciences, 2018. Vol. 41, no. 10, pp. 651—653. DOI:10.1016/j.tins.2018.08.004
- Bueti D., Walsh V. The parietal cortex and the representation of time, space, number and other magnitudes. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 2009. Vol. 364, no. 1525, pp. 1831—1840. DOI:10.1098/ rstb.2009.0028
- Ding X., Feng N., He T., Cheng X., Fan Z. Can mental time lines co-exist in 3D space? Acta Psychologica, 2020. Vol. 207, no. 1, article ID 103084, 13 p. DOI:10.1016/j.actpsy.2020.103084
- Onoe H., Komori M., Onoe K., Takechi H., Tsukada H., Watanabe Y. Cortical networks recruited for time perception: A monkey positron emission tomography (PET) study. Neuroimage, 2001. Vol. 13, no. 1, pp. 37—45. DOI:10.1006/ nimg.2000.0670
- De Tommaso M., Prpic V. Slow and fast beat sequences are represented differently through space. Attention, Perception, Psychophys, 2020. Vol. 82, pp. 2765—2773. DOI:10.3758/s13414-019-01945-8
- Dehaene S., Bossini S., Giraux P. The Mental Representation of Parity and Number Magnitude. Experimental Psychology: General, 1993. Vol. 122, no. 3, pp. 371—396. DOI:10.1037/0096-3445.122.3.371
- He D., He X., Zhao T., Wang J., Li L., Louwerse M. Does Number Perception Cause Automatic Shifts of Spatial Attention? A Study of the Att-SNARC Effect in Numbers and Chinese Months. Frontiers in Psychology, 2020. Vol. 11, article ID 680, 13 p. DOI:10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00680
- Dormal V., Seron X., Pesenti M. Numerosity-duration interference: A Stroop experiment. Acta Psychologica, 2006. Vol. 121, no. 2, pp. 109—124. DOI:10.1016/j.actpsy.2005.06.003
- Agostino C. S., Zana Y., Balci F., Claessens P. M. E. Effect of presentation format on judgment of long-range time intervals. Frontiers in Psychology, 2019. Vol. 10, article ID 1479, 14 p. DOI:10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01479
- Gentner D., Imai M., Boroditsky L. As time goes by: Evidence for two systems in processing space — time metaphors. Language and Cognitive Processes, 2002. Vol. 17, no. 5, pp. 537—565. DOI:10.1080/01690960143000317
- Gevers W., Reynvoet B., Fias W. The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized. Cognition, 2003. Vol. 87, pp. B87—B95. DOI:10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00234-2
- Gevers W., Reynvoet B., Fias W. The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized: evidence from days of the week. Cortex, 2004. Vol. 40, no. 1, pp. B87—B95. DOI:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70938-9
- Hartmann M., Mast F. W. Moving along the mental time line influences the processing of future related words. Consciousness and Cognition, 2012. Vol. 21, no. 3, pp. 1558—1562. DOI:10.1016/j.concog.2012.06.015
- Ishihara M., Keller P. E., Rossetti Y., Prinz W. Horizontal spatial representations of time: Evidence for the STEARC effect. Cortex, 2008. Vol. 44, no. 4, pp. 454—461. DOI:10.1016/j.cortex.2007.08.010
- Fuhrman O., Mccormick K., Chen E., Jiang H., Shu D., Mao S., Boroditsky L. How Linguistic and Cultural Forces Shape Conceptions of Time: English and Mandarin Time in 3D. Cognitive Science A Multidisciplinary, 2011. Vol. 35, no. 7, pp. 1305—1328. DOI:10.1111/j.1551-6709.2011.01193.x
- Kato T., Imaizumi S., Tanno Y. Metaphorical action retrospectively but not prospectively alters emotional judgment. Frontiers in Psychology, 2018. Vol. 9, article ID 1927, 10 p. DOI:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01927
- Lakoff G., Johnson M. Metaphors We Live By. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1980, 242 p.
- Assmus A., Marshall J. C., Ritzl A., Noth J., Zilles K., Fink G. R. Left inferior parietal cortex integrates time and space during collision judgments. Neuroimage, 2003. Vol. 20, supplement 1, pp. S82—S88. DOI:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.09.025
- Leon M. I., Shadlen M. N. Representation of Time by Neurons in the Posterior Parietal Cortex of the Macaque tures may also play a role in time perception (Harrington et al As a first step toward elucidating the repre). Neuron, 2003. Vol. 38, no. 2.pp. 317—327. DOI:10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00185-5
- Makioka S. Idiosyncratic spatial representations of the days of the week in individuals without synesthesia. Cognition, 2021. Vol. 207, article ID 104500, 21 p. DOI:10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104500
- Winter B., Matlock T., Shaki S., Fischer M. H. Mental number space in three dimensions. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 2015. Vol. 57, pp. 209—219. DOI:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.09.005
- Anelli F., Peters-Founshtein G., Shreibman Y., Moreh E., Forlani C., Frassinetti F., Arzy S. Nature and nurture effects on the spatiality of the mental time line. Scientific Reports, 2018. Vol. 8, article ID 11710, 10 p. DOI:10.1038/s41598-018- 29584-3
- Nourouzi Mehlabani S., Sabaghypour S., Nazari M. A. Number is special: time, space, and number interact in a temporal reproduction task. Cognitive Processing, 2020. Vol. 21, no. 3, pp. 449—459. DOI:10.1007/s10339-020-00968-6
- Núñez R. E., Sweetser E. With the future behind them: Convergent evidence from Aymara language and gesture in the crosslinguistic comparison of spatial construals of time. Cognitive science, 2006. Vol. 30, no. 3, pp. 401—450. DOI:10.1207/ s15516709cog0000_62
- Velasco C., Adams C., Petit O., Spence C. On the localization of tastes and tasty products in 2D space. Food Quality and Preference, 2019. Vol. 71, pp. 438—446. DOI:10.1016/j.foodqual.2018.08.018
- Pacini A. M., Barnard P. J. Exocentric coding of the mapping between valence and regions of space: Implications for embodied cognition. Acta Psychologica, 2021. Vol. 214, article ID 103264, 6 p. DOI:10.1016/j.actpsy.2021.103264
- Pitt B., Casasanto D. The correlations in experience principle: How culture shapes concepts of time and number. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2020. Vol. 149, no. 6, pp. 1048—1070. DOI:10.1037/xge0000696
- Podwysocki C., Reeve R. A., Forte J. D. The importance of ordinal information in interpreting number/letter line data. Frontiers in Psychology, 2019. Vol. 10, article ID 692, 10 p. DOI:10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00692
- Prete G. Spatializing Emotions Besides Magnitudes: Is There a Left-to-Right Valence or Intensity Mapping? Symmetry, 2020. Vol. 12, no. 5, article ID 775, 13 p. DOI:10.3390/sym12050775
- Ristic J., Wright A., Kingstone A. The number line effect reflects top-down control. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2006. Vol. 13, no. 5, pp. 862—868. DOI:10.3758/BF03194010
- Rodríguez L. “Time is not a line.” Temporal gestures in Chol Mayan. Journal of Pragmatics, 2019. Vol. 151, pp. 1—17. DOI:10.1016/j.pragma.2019.07.003
- Sell A. J., Kaschak M. P. Processing time shifts affects the execution of motor responses. Brain and Language, 2011. Vol. 117, no. 1, pp. 39—44. DOI:10.1016/j.bandl.2010.07.003
- Prpic V., Soranzo A., Santoro I., Fantoni C., Galmonte A., Agostini T., Murgia M. SNARC-like compatibility effects for physical and phenomenal magnitudes: a study on visual illusions. Psychological Research, 2020. Vol. 84, pp. 950—965. DOI:10.1007/s00426-018-1125-1
- Srinivasan M., Carey S. The Long and the Short of it: On the Nature and Origin of Functional Overlap Between Representations of Space and Time. Cognition, 2010. Vol. 116, no. 2, 40 p. DOI:10.1016/j.cognition.2010.05.005
- Stavy R., Tirosh D. How Students (Mis-)Understand Science and Mathematics: Intuitive Rules. New York: Teachers’ College Press, 2000, 127 p.
- Sun J., Zhang Q. How do Mandarin speakers conceptualize time? Beyond the horizontal and vertical dimensions. Cognitive Processing, 2021. Vol. 22, no. 2, pp. 171—181. DOI:10.1007/s10339-020-00987-3
- Teghil A., Marc I. B., Boccia M. Mental representation of autobiographical memories along the sagittal mental timeline: Evidence from spatiotemporal interference. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2021. Vol. 28, no. 4, pp. 1327—1335. DOI:10.3758/s13423-021-01906-z
- Myachykov A., Scheepers C., Fischer M. H., Kessler K. TEST: A tropic, embodied, and situated theory of cognition. Topics in Cognitive Science, 2014. Vol. 6, pp. 442—460. DOI:10.1111/tops.12024
- Zhao T., He X., Zhao X., Huang J., Zhang W., Wu S., Chen Q. The influence of time units on the flexibility of the spatial numerical association of response codes effect. British Journal of Psychology, 2018. Vol. 109, no. 2, pp. 299—320. DOI:10.1111/bjop.12273
- Tillman K. A., Tulagan N., Fukuda E., Barner D. The mental timeline is gradually constructed in childhood. Developmental Science, 2018. Vol. 21. № 6. P. 1—12. DOI:10.1111/desc.12679
- Beracci A., Rescott M. L., Natale V., Fabbri M. The vertical space—time association. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 2022. Vol. 75(9), pp. 1674—1693. DOI:10.1177/17470218211057031
- Leone M. J., Salles A., Pulver A., Golombek D. A., Sigman M. Time drawings: Spatial representation of temporal concepts. Consciousness and Cognition, 2018. Vol. 59, pp. 10—25. DOI:10.1016/j.concog.2018.01.005
- Burns P., McCormack T., Jaroslawska A. J., O’Connor P. A., Caruso E. M. Time Points: A Gestural Study of the Development of Space-Time Mappings. Cognitive Science A Multidisciplinary, 2019. Vol. 43, no. 12 Article ID, e12801, 24 p. DOI:10.1111/cogs.12801
- Topić V., Stojić S., Domijan D. An implicit task reveals space-time associations along vertical and diagonal axes. Psychological Research, 2022. Vol. 86, 10 p. DOI:10.1007/s00426-021-01561-y
- Torralbo A., Santiago J., Lupiáñez J. Flexible conceptual projection of time onto spatial frames of reference. Cognitive science, 2006. Vol. 30, no.pp. 745—757. DOI:10.1207/s15516709cog0000_67
- Tversky B., Kugelmass S., Winter A. Cross-cultural and developmental trends in graphic productions. Cognitive Psychology, 1991. Vol. 23, no. 4, pp. 515—557. DOI:10.1016/0010-0285(91)90005-9
- Walsh V. A theory of magnitude: Common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2003. Vol. 7, no. 11, pp. 483—488. DOI:10.1016/j.tics.2003.09.002
- Spatola N., Santiago J., Beffara B., Mermillod M., Ferrand L., Ouellet M. When the sad past is left: The mental metaphors between time, valence, and space. Frontiers in Psychology, 2018. Vol. 9, article ID 1019, 17 p. DOI:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01019
- Ulrich R., Eikmeier V., de la Vega I., Fernandez S. R., Alex-Ruf S., Maienborn C. With the past behind and the future ahead: Back-to-front representation of past and future sentences. Memory & Cognition, 2012. Vol. 40, pp. 483—495. DOI:10.3758/s13421-011-0162-4
- Woodin G., Winter B. Placing abstract concepts in space: Quantity, time and emotional valence. Frontiers in Psychology, 2018. Vol. 9, article ID 2169, 14 p. DOI:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02169
- Xiao C., Zhao M., Chen L. Both Earlier Times and the Future Are “Front”: The Distinction Between Time- and Ego-Reference-Points in Mandarin Speakers’ Temporal Representation. Cognitive science, 2018. Vol. 42, no. 3, pp. 1026— 1040. DOI:10.1111/cogs.12552
- Zhang M., Hudson J. A. Children’s understanding of yesterday and tomorrow. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2018. Vol. 170, pp. 107—133. DOI:10.1016/j.jecp.2018.01.010
Information About the Authors
Metrics
Views
Total: 486
Previous month: 13
Current month: 4
Downloads
Total: 128
Previous month: 4
Current month: 0