Cultural-Historical Psychology
2025. Vol. 21, no. 1, 34–41
doi:10.17759/chp.2025210104
ISSN: 1816-5435 / 2224-8935 (online)
Overcoming the Semantic Barrier and Developing the Ability to Use Communicative Tools. Part II. Psychotechnique
Abstract
This article describes the psychotechnique of mediating (real-life, communicative) action in working with difficult children. We outline the stages and corresponding environments leading to turning points in the formation of consciousness[1] (mental model) of a child with ASD. It’s defined by restructuring interfunctional relations in the process of mediation and developing conceptual reflective interfunctional thinking (not only reflective functional thinking). The psychotechnique can be used in practice, both in work with difficult children and in general education in order to prevent the motivational disorders and to raise the age boundary by restructuring the performance of yet unformed function).
General Information
Keywords: developmental psychology, experimental genetic method, mediating (real-life, communicative) action, psychotechnique, activity-based approach in education, emotional and volitional disorders, autism spectrum disorders, semantic barrier, semantic shift, restructuring the performance of the impaired function, position, conscious interfunctional thinking
Journal rubric: Theory and Methodology
Article type: scientific article
DOI: https://doi.org/10.17759/chp.2025210104
Received: 11.04.2024
Accepted:
For citation: Soldatenkova E.N. Overcoming the Semantic Barrier and Developing the Ability to Use Communicative Tools. Part II. Psychotechnique. Kul'turno-istoricheskaya psikhologiya = Cultural-Historical Psychology, 2025. Vol. 21, no. 1, pp. 34–41. DOI: 10.17759/chp.2025210104.
Full text
The proof comes nearer, nearer,
What man praises in deepest Nature,
Through Reason we dare to probe it,
And what she organizes, here,
We’re now able to crystallize it.
Faust, by Goethe
The development of practical psychotechniques, as follows from Part I of this study, is both historically and methodologically well-grounded, meeting the demand of our time, namely the need to enhance work with children with an appropriate method that forms the child’s subjectivity. After all, attempts to copy foreign methods have not solved this problem whatsoever. While they are reminiscent of the situation described earlier by L.S. Vygotsky [10, p. 78]: “annexation — the mechanical transfer of one system’s pieces into another — in this instance, as always, seems almost miraculous and testifies to the truth…,” that is not quite the case. Replication of foreign methods and their introduction into educational practice has exposed the crisis in psychology that L.S. Vygotsky predicted as early as in 1927 [14]. And the issue here is “not the lack of material, which is abundantly collected, and not the lack of philosophical and methodological principles, which are abundant as well, but the fact that practitioners find it impossible to comprehend the entire spectrum of this variability and to treat the replicated practices critically and compare these data with methodological psychology.” “There is not a single science where theory and practice have followed such different paths (L. Binswanger, 1922, p. 6)” [10, p. 125].
The system of teaching children with ASD, despite the efforts expended, still does not meet the performance goals set by the Russian state standards in terms of creating conditions for the formation of personal and meta-disciplinary results. The reason is that the spontaneously formed “modern scientific evidence-based practices”3 are far removed from the actual development of the child’s subjectivity and do not allow the child to achieve what they are truly capable of through the construction of alternative development paths [8, p. 105]. Development of psychotechniques for mediating (real-life, communicative) action becomes “the defining point of the circle” and “the main driving force of the crisis in its last phase” [10, p. 138], if we are to recall another idea expressed by L.S. Vygotsky: “the path of development,” in this case education for children with ASD, “is not always straight, but takes twists and turns…” [24, p. 180].
Returning to subjectivity as the center of L.S. Vygotsky’s theory and the practical impact of psychotechniques, it is necessary to note that subjectivity emerges when the transition from the natural form to the cultural form is expressed: “the subject and subjectivity are believed cannot be pointed at”; they are not actually physically present. Vygotsky does not point to the individual (or group of individuals), but to their way(s) of life. The subject is constructed as the mutual transition between the real and ideal forms. The pattern behind this process can be described as “sign mediation,” “when naturally formed stereotypes of behavior become the subject of change and … are overcome, becoming conscious and arbitrary” (i.e., a person masters their own behavior) [25, p. 8, 9]. The main condition for planning this transition is interfunctional restructurisation, achieved in a way that shapes the child’s cultural development. After all, “the only way … is to construct … a process with given properties” [12, p. 306]. Of course, the conditions of organizing real-life communicative action are not outlined explicitly, but they can be reconstructed from the works of Russian researchers. The need to read and understand these texts hermeneutically causes difficulties, which are avoided through replicating the readily available foreign techniques. We have made an attempt to describe these conditions. Taken together, they should create a developmental space, accommodating the child’s affective reactions and meeting the following requirements:
— introducing new things in “doses”;
— changing the environment (nature of relationships) in the process of the child’s (the social environment’s) absorption of cultural experience, stage-by-stage [26]4; note that these stages must involve transition from reflecting objects (social environment) to trying out the limits of controlling their behavior and forming the skill of rationally using the communicative means (directly or, in the absence of verbal communication, indirectly [8, p.105], through restructuring the performance of the impaired function) or the mode of action [1]; overcoming syncretic thinking and developing interfunctional thinking;
— considering the specifics of how the child contacts the social environment and experiences social relations; helping the child overcome the semantic barrier (defined as the child’s insensitivity to the adult’s educational influence [23, p. 9], although it is rather the adult who is insensitive to manifestations of the child’s activity);
— allocating the child’s personal action with its transformation into the child’s “I-action” [28, p. 163], building work with the child with a focus on their spontaneous activity, helping the child form their personal stance;
— shaping positional vision in a child with ASD, correlated with the way of seeing the Other, i.e., the carrier of the concept of action [25], which would help the child to change their mode of action and alter the social situation when reconstructing human culture.
It is reasonable to consider the development of mediating (real-life, communicative) action on the basis of three subsystems of conditions that ensure its formation:
the first subsystem contains conditions that allow to consistently transform the child’s personal action into the I-action, forming the regulatory component of activity;
the second subsystem contains conditions that prevent the child’s demotivation, the deterioration of their emotional status and the negative emotional response to failure;
the third subsystem contains conditions that allow the child to meaningfully use their communicative means (directly or, as an alternative, indirectly, by restructuring the performance of the impaired function) [12, p. 168].
It should also be specified that the success of such work can be ensured by compliance with two requirements:
- The specialist applying the psychotechnique must have interdisciplinary knowledge of medical psychology in child and adolescent psychiatry, developmental psychology, systemic family therapy, and speech therapy (neurology).
- The specialist must develop the skill of holistic systemic interfunctional process analysis (as opposed to the simple structural functional behavioral analysis of external manifestations), which will allow them to restructure the performance of impaired in a fundamentally different way, preventing an increase in affective symptomatology, known as “the seven-star constellation of crisis symptoms” [9, p. 374]. Accounting for these requirements will make it possible to approach the process of the child’s development in a holistic way, to go above and beyond imposing “an image of the desired future” onto the child [22, p. 73], which would only entangle the child in “a vain struggle, eternal self-deception” [22, p. 76], and to actually form new phenomena of an entirely different quality: speech communication, I-action, self-reflection (i.e., criticism of performance), meaningful action (as opposed to mirrored repetition), with self-control and self-correction, ultimately helping the child gain an open, conscious attitude to the complex, contradictory and not always orderly world.
Considering that the degree of mastering an action is measured by its completeness, and the measure of generalization does not always allow for an immediate execution of a complete action, the triune task (the triune subsystem of conditions) is not always maintained in communication with the child, and adults limit themselves to setting only one task — to develop the skill of using a communicative tool, and through alternative communication at that (as opposed to speech), we suggest a preliminary “action plan for our work and immediately proceed to the solution of our tasks according to this plan” [12, p. 189], thus maintaining proper guidelines and keeping communication at a higher level (fig.).
This research was carried out in sequential stages, each of the stages being characterized by its own content (type of interfunctional relations, social situation) and the specifics of the mediator’s actions during the transition (crisis) periods. In the process of constructing the general field of action, consistently moving to a higher degree of generalization, it is expedient to include the missing subsystems of conditions in the analysis of the child’s adult caretakers: “As soon as what has been learned has acquired a reduced form… an opportunity opens up for us to concentrate on the new task” [12, p. 303]. The action plan is an extremely convenient medium for this, because it allows us, by maintaining a higher degree of generalization, to open up the possibility of action variability within each of the subsystems, while eliminating unnecessary non-directed movements that lead to disorganization. This directly correlates with the stages of development of thinking and play according to L.S. Vygotsky [7, p. 163], and also with D.B. Elkonin’s age periodization [30, p. 487]. Let us consider these stages in more detail.
The first stage: Pre-speech thinking and pre-intellectual play. [15, p. 156], mastering relationships [30, p. 487]. Most often corresponds to the primary reception, when the directed interview of the child’s adult caretakers problematizes the discrepancy between the actual means of caring for the child with the cultural (ideal) means. Traditionally, mistakes occur in all three subsystems of conditions, but especially often the conditions of the second and third subsystems are not considered, which causes the child to form a specific type of reaction, reflected thinking, and mechanical will, and to alternative communication only.
The second stage: The greatest discovery in life is the inability to think. [15, p. 156], crisis of the social development situation [30, p .487]. The work is based on the child’s actions in everyday life situations without the use of instrumental learning with a system of motivation. The main task solved at this stage is the primary orientation in the boundary of the child’s achievements and in the conditions of caring for the child; the limitations of these tools are demonstrated, and fundamental guidelines are laid down for future child-adult action (with ongoing operational control by the specialist).
The third stage: The meeting as an instrumental method. Stage of cultural reception. [15, p. 156], mastering the subject [30, p. 487]. Formulation of the triune task, work with reference to the cultural norm and action concept (model), as well as the image of the action situation, formation a positional vision in the child (adult caretakers) in the process of learning to use the communicative means (mode of action): direct and indirect paths, with subsequent dosage-sequential reorganization of relations, formulation of the final requirements imposed on the executive part of action. Pre-check of the action comprehension level: necessary to assess the understanding to what degree the mode of action has been mastered. Formally we distinguish three types of action comprehension [21]:
- reflective, formal: imitation of action (mirroring), mechanical repetition;
- reflexive: comprehension within one functional system and orientation in one subsystem of conditions;
- interfunctional.
The fourth stage: Speech thinking: the external tool becomes internal, and is intellectualized. [15, p. 157], crisis of the adult-child relationship [30, с. 487]. This stage is characterized by the third interfunctional type of comprehension of the material, when participants choose meaningful components of action in a given situation, rely on other meaningful components when the situation changes, or complete the actions of other people. Fluent mastery of the communicative action is observed at this stage.
Following D.B. Elkonin, we note the presence of critical periods in the process of action construction. They are characterized by conditional boundaries due to the child’s and the adult caretakers’ desire for independent judgment and the emergence of affect in response to the actions of the mediator. They always accompany “play as interaction between the real and ideal forms and, hence, as a special form of assimilation” [30, p. 483] and demand attention to the occurrence of insufficiency and contradiction conflicts that lead to symptom aggravation and growth of “the seven-star constellation of crisis symptoms” [9, p. 374]. Therefore, the child’s response to the adult, active involvement a culturally affirming normative action, and reflection on this action, must be retained consistently at all stages of support work. After all, the child’s retention of the action’s concept (without disorganization on their part), is determined by the teacher’s action: “the direct focus on the real object conditions of action (what A.N. Leontiev called operation) is included and defined in the cumulative action by focusing on the action of another! I act in such a way as to organize and prepare the actions of another person. I hold a nail with one hand and hammer it in with the other. My hands are positioned in a way that makes hammering easier. The two hands are like two people. This is what the inter-psychic is all about. That’s what’s getting intergrated! The Other is getting integrated!” [30, p. 518]. At the same time, support is needed not only for the children, but also for the parents, especially in the situation of caring for children with a complex disability structure. Parents of such childrens are more likely to lose motivation, buying into the idea that it is impossible to change the trajectory of their child’s development. It is important to trace these situations and translate the dialogue into a reflexive attitude to the mode of action (the mastery of the psychotechnical form), emphasizing what may have passed unnoticed. In children, just as in adults, disorganization is exacerbated by reflected syncretic thinking. When communicating with them, you can easily notice when they are echoing phrases said by their parents or specialists, even with the preservation of the original intonation. Therefore, in order not to further disorganize the child, it is necessary to understand what they “absorbed from the whole context” [6, p. 347]. In child development, “word” and “context” merge together, and without specially organized work, “special ways of including the actions of one person in the actions of another” [27, p. 60], “word” may never acquire the semantic shades of human culture, as “…a thought that is not embodied in the word remains a Stygian shadow, nothing but fog” [6, p. 361]. Let us confirm this with practical examples. We shall describe the effects that emerge while working with a simple reflexological action (behavioral approach) and mediating (real-life, communicative) action.
Work within the limits of reflexological action was distinguished by being instrumental, failing to consider the context and individual history of the child’s development, and using the functional analysis of behavior and external manifestations. The construction of the second type of communicative situation supported its dynamics, as well as changes in the child’s position, and restructured intrafunctional connections and relations, leading to disinhibition of the speech function and development of the regulatory component of the child’s activity. As expected, we got different results.
While work within the framework of the reflexological communicative action yielded certain results (unsustainable in the long term), it also had a delayed negative effect: children in the control group still showed echo reactions, insufficient speech development, lack of orientation in a situation, and fixed forms of behavior and were focused only on the emotional component of action (the adult’s facial expressions and gestures), without analyzing what the adult was actually saying to them, even if it was in conflict with their interests. Here is a concrete example, which is more convincing than statistical calculations: the specialist consults a boy, studying in grade 2 of the 8.2 program5. In an interaction situation, he is restless and calms down by drawing the alphabet with English letters, which he can continue doing indefinitely until stopped. He perceives the activity specifically as drawing, as he does not know how to spell and makes numerous notable mistakes (mirrors when writing, misses parts of a letter…). He follows situational instructions, despite his pronounced problems with speech comprehension. In a situation where communication is initiated, he shows an increase in anxiety and disorganized behavior due to a lack of understanding of what is required of him. The child looks at the adult, tries to choose an action that will satisfy the adult, going over the previously required actions, doing one or the other, repeating them in a stereotypical way: taking an eraser, erasing, putting it away in a pencil case, pushing a chair. Every time, he looks into the adult’s eyes, as if asking: “Am I doing it right? Is this what you want from me?” In the absence of an emotional reaction from the adult (approval or critique), the child’s disorganization only intensifies. He tries to say some words, turning to the adult and trying to initiate a dialogue: “album,” “write,” starts to rock in the chair and cry, grabs the specialist’s hand as a plea to organize his own activities (give “physical cues” in the words of “ABA therapists”). If an adult does not give the boy any examples to follow, he gets even more disorganized. In a situation where the instruction diverges from the child’s interests, there is also profound disorganization. For example, the specialist offers the child to ride a scooter (knowing that the child does not like it). Instead of answering, “No, I don’t want to!” which would have satisfied the researcher, the child begins to run around in a frenzy, crying and screaming: “The scooter! The scooter!” After the adult says, “You don’t want to ride the scooter? You don’t have to ride the scooter,” the child, still crying, takes the scooter and heads for the door with it, intending to perform an action they are not interested in and were not even required to do. This example perfectly demonstrates the absence of developmental dynamics in the child during the preschool period, both at the level of using a cultural tool and at the level of organizing their actions. The cause is the instrumental, reflexological (behavioral) nature of working with the child.
When a psychotechnique is applied instead, children reach a level of meaningful communication and no longer make the mistakes typical for children that worked with reflexological practices. These children are more confident in using a more universal cultural tool (verbal communication), reaching the level of word creation and independent word formation when trying to construct a phrase. They have a decreased echo response. Thus, they can already correct the specialist when they use “trick” tasks in diagnostics, and do not lose control when frustrated. When playing together, they can be heard saying, “This is blueberry ice cream, not chocolate ice cream.” These children, as well as their parents, already become less sensitive to disturbances and less disorganized. A little girl’s mother would say, “Ah, I see what you want,” with a smile on her face. This phrase serves as excellent proof that a shared field of meaning had formed between the specialist and the child’s parents: the field of understanding, the fullest generalized reference form of communicative action.
Through these examples, we clearly demonstrate that the communicative model, having been enriched with additional substantive parameters and having evolved beyond the superficial view of communication as an analysis of only external manifestations, forms a qualitatively different new phenomenon: reflexive speech consciousness, as opposed to reflected thinking. After all, “the capabilities … of imitation6 are not infinite” [9, p. 263], and the only condition allowing children with ASD to leave the autistic world is semantic restructuring: what we need is “not mechanical, automatic, meaningless imitation, but reasonable imitation … based on understanding” [9, p. 263]. For specialists, the communicative process, correlating with the meaning of the situation, changing its form in accordance with the emotional intuition and anticipation (completeness of orientation: interfunctional analysis), must become the intuition of conscience (as A.A. Ukhtomsky called it), which, “like Princess Cinderella… works more efficiently than her sister, the Will.” [17, p. 334]. This is when a child with emotional and volitional disorders (even with ASD) can obtain a different kind of knowledge of the world: “The emergence of a new point of development… here the affect that prompts the child to overcome difficulties is extremely important. If these difficulties do not demagnetize the child… but activate them, they lead reveal an alternative development route” [8, p. 127]. Though, of course, this cannot be done alone, since “it is impossible for one person, but possible for two” [6, p. 361]. And “workarounds” need to be used as well [8, p. 105]. To read the Word [6, p. 361] means to understand the Meaning, which will reveal the line of reasoning used by L. S. Vygotsky when talking about the relationship between affect and intellect. And only then will “…this very notion (affective ‘defect’), a sure sign of our own defect, disappear” [8, p. 72]. We must measure not what has been done, but “WHAT HAS NOT BEEN DONE YET” [15, p. 565]. This is what ensures a change in attitude to the world, an exit from the clinical paradigm and withdrawal of the cultural-historical theory from the ranks of behavioral sciences.
1 Russian science traditionally believes that, aside from steady evolution in the development of the human psyche, there are also leaps forward: the purposeful work and clear organization of the environment in order to make it possible to achieve changes in the child's psyche and significantly accelerate the child's mental development. This, for example, is covered in research on the crisis nature of development, in D.B. Elkonin's age periodization.
2 В традиции отечественной науки считается, что возможны не только эволюционные периоды в развитии психики, но скачкообразные — поворотные периоды, позволяющие при целенаправленной работе и четкой организации среды добиться изменений в психике ребенка и существенно ускорить его психическое развитие. Такой позиции придерживается, например, Д.Б. Эльконин в исследовании о кризисном характере развития в возрастной периодизации.
3 В Is this science? After all, any science explains the genesis of a phenomenon and focuses on historicism and methodology, but what do we have here? Just statistics. In his work Questions of the Theory and History of Psychology [19, p. 454], L.S. Vygotsky called this a "feldsher's approach to science"— author's note.
4 In case of children with ASD, this involves creating a favorable environment for overcoming the semantic barrier and gaining the ability to understand speech; furthermore, this involves training the child to use communicative means (by taking the "the direct route" and the "indirect route" with nonverbal children, through restructuring the performance of the impaired function).
5 It begs the question, HOW did a nonverbal child get into this training program?
6 As opposed to overcoming the hypotonia of one's consciousness and the ability to go beyond reflective syncretic thinking and functional analysis. After all, turning to the tradition of non-classical psychology — the classical studies of domestic knowledge — makes it possible to build "togetherness" not upon the intersection of actions (the subject conflict), but rather upon overcoming the conflict of meaning and preventing disorganized behavior.
References
- Alekhina S.V. Soldatenkova E.N. Tekhnologiya razvitiya kommunikatsii detei s RAS kak sostavlyayushchaya inklyuzivnoi praktiki [Technology for developing communication in children with ASD as a component of inclusive practice]. In Alekhina S.V. (ed.), Inklyuzivnoe obrazovanie: Rezul'taty, opyt i perspektivy: Sbornik materialov III Mezhdunarodnoi nauchno-prakticheskoi konferentsii [Inclusive education: Results, experience and prospects: Proceedings of the 3rd International research-to-practice conference]. Moscow: Publ. Moscow State University of Psyсhology and Education, 2015, pp. 470—478. (In Russ.)
- Arkhipov B.A. El'konin B.D. Yazyk antropotekhnicheskogo (posrednicheskogo) deistviya [The language of anthropotechnical (intermediary) action]. In El'konin B.D. (ed.), Antropopraksis: Ezhegodnik gumanitarnykh issledovanii: 3. [Anthropopraxis: Humanities Research Yearbook: Vol. 3]. Izhevsk: Publ. ERGO, 2011, pp. 5—16. (In Russ.)
- Bel'tyukov V.I. Triada kak faktor sistemoobrazovaniya [Triad as a factor of system formation]. Defektologiya [Defectology], 2000, no. 5, pp. 3—8. (In Russ.)
- Veresov N.N. Experimental-Genetic Method and Psychology of Consciousness: In Search of the Lost (Part One). Kul'turno-istoricheskaya psikhologiya = Cultural-Historical Psychology, 2014. Vol. 10, no. 4, pp. 121—130. (In Russ.)
- Vygotskii L.S. Sobranie sochinenii: v 6 t. T. 1. Voprosy teorii i istorii psikhologii [Collected works: in 6 vol.: Vol. 1. Questions of theory and history of psychology]. Luriya A.R. (eds.). Moscow: Publ. Pedagogika, 1982. 488 p. (In Russ.)
- Vygotskii L.S. Sobranie sochinenii: v 6 t. T. 2. Problemy obshchei psikhologii [Collected works: in 6 vol.: Vol. 2. Issues of General Psychology]. Davydov V.V. (ed.). Moscow: Publ. Pedagogika, 1982. 504 p. (In Russ.)
- Vygotskii L.S. Sobranie sochinenii: v 6 t. T. 3. Problemy razvitiya psikhiki [Collected works: in 6 vol.: Vol. 3. Problems of Psychic Development]. Matyushkin A.M. (ed.). Moscow: Publ. Pedagogika, 1983. 368 p. (In Russ.)
- Vygotskii L.S. Sobranie sochinenii: v 6 t. T. 5. Osnovy defektologii [Collected works: in 6 vol.: Vol. 5. The Basis of Defectology] / comp. by T.A. Vlasova. Moscow: Publ. Pedagogika, 1983. 368 p. (In Russ.)
- Vygotskii L.S. Sobranie sochinenii: v 6 t. T. 4. Detskaya psikhologiya [Collected works: in 6 vol.: Vol. 4. Child Psychology]. El'konin D.B (ed.). Moscow: Publ. Pedagogika, 1984. 432 p. (In Russ.)
- Vygotskii L.S. Psikhologiya razvitiya cheloveka [Human developmental psychology]. Moscow: Publ. Eksmo: Publ. Smysl, 2005. 1136 p. (In Russ.)
- Galilei G. Dialog o dvukh glavneishikh sistemakh mira [Dialogue Concerning the Two Chief World Systems]. Dolgov A.I. (ed.). Moscow: Publ. RIPOL Klassik, 2018. 918 p. (In Russ.)
- Gal'perin P.Ya. Lektsii po psikhologii: uchebnoe posobie dlya studentov vuzov [Psychology Lectures: study guide for university students]. Moscow: Publ. Knizhnyi dom “Universitet”: Publ. Vysshaya shkola, 2002. 400 p. (In Russ.)
- Davydov V.V. Vidy obobshcheniya v obuchenii: Logiko-psikhologicheskie problemy postroeniya uchebnykh predmetov [Types of generalization in learning: Logical and psychological problems of constructing study subjects]. Moscow: Publ. Pedagogika, 1972. 404 p. (In Russ.)
- Zavershneva E.Yu. Issledovanie rukopisi L.S. Vygotskogo “Istoricheskii smysl psikhologicheskogo krizisa” [Study of L.S. Vygotsky’s manuscript “The historical meaning of the psychological crisis”]. Voprosy psikhologii [Psychology Issues], 2009, no. 6, pp. 119—137. (In Russ.)
- Zavershneva E., van der Veer R. (eds.) Zapisnye knizhki L.S. Vygotskogo: Izbrannoe [L.S. Vygotsky’s notebooks: Selected excerpts]. Moscow: Publ. Kanon+, 2017. 608 p. (In Russ.)
- Zakharov A.I. Preduprezhdenie otklonenii v povedenii rebenka [Prevention of deviations in child behavior]. 3rd ed., corrected. Saint Petersburg: Publ. Soyuz: Publ. Lenizdat, 2000. 222 p. (In Russ.)
- Zinchenko V.P. Psikhologiya predmetnogo deistviya [Psychology of objective action]. Moscow: Publ. Tsentr gumanitarnykh initsiativ, 2018. 856 p. (In Russ.)
- Luriya A.R. Priroda chelovecheskikh konfliktov: Ob"ektivnoe izuchenie dezorganizatsii povedeniya cheloveka [The Nature of Human Conflict: An Objective Study of the Disorganization of Human Behavior]. Belopol'skii V.I. (ed.). Moscow: Publ. Kogito-Tsentr, 2002. 527 p. (In Russ.)
- Il'enkov E.V. <Lektsiya o soznanii> [Lectures on the consciousness] / Ed., foreword and footnotes by A.D. Maidanskii. Filosofskii zhurnal [Philosophy Journal], 2021, vol. 14, no. 1, pp. 162‒172. (In Russ.) DOI:10.21146/2072-0726-2021-14-1-162-172
- Melas V.B. Shest' sposobov sushchestvovaniya [Six ways of existence]. Logiko-filosofskie shtudii [Logical-Philosophical Studies], 2016, no. 14, pp. 224—231. (In Russ.)
- Nezhnov P.G. Testy SAM (Student Achievement Monitoring) v obrazovatel'noi praktike [SAM (Student Achievement Monitoring) tests in educational practice]. Moscow: Publ. Avtorskii klub, 2018. 48 p. (In Russ.)
- Pinskii A.A. Liberal'naya ideya i praktika obrazovaniya [Liberal idea and practice of education]. Moscow: Publ. Higher School of Economics, 2007. 667 p. (In Russ.)
- Slavina L.S. Trudnye deti [Difficult children]. Chudnovskii V.E. (ed.). Voronezh: Publ. MODEK; Moscow: Publ. Institut prakticheskoi psikhologii, 1998. 448 p. (In Russ.)
- Sukhareva G.E. Klinika psikhopatii vtoroi gruppy (Prodolzhenie): Lektsiya 19 [Clinical formulation of second group psychopathies (Continuation): Lecture 19]. In Klinicheskie lektsii po psikhiatrii detskogo vozrasta: [v 3 t.]: T. 2 [Clinical lectures on child psychology: in 3 vol.: Vol. 2]. Moscow: Publ. Medgiz, 1959, pp. 278—286. (In Russ.)
- El'konin B.D. Vvedenie v psikhologiyu razvitiya (v traditsii kul'turno-istoricheskoi teorii L.S. Vygotskogo) [Introduction to developmental psychology (in the tradition of the cultural-historical theory of L.S.Vygotsky)]. Moscow: Publ. Trivola, 1994. 168 p. (In Russ.)
- El'konin B.D. O fenomenakh perekhodnykh form deistviya [On the phenomena of transitional forms of action]. Voprosy psikhologii [Psychology Issues], 1994, no. 1, pp. 47—54. (In Russ.)
- El'konin B.D. L. S. Vygotskii — D. B. El'konin: znakovoe oposredstvovanie i sovokupnoe deistvie [L. S. Vygotsky — D. B. El'konin: sign mediation and cumulative action]. Voprosy psikhologii [Psychology Issues], 1996, no. 5, pp. 57—63. (In Russ.)
- El'konin B.D. Oposredstvovanie: Deistvie: Razvitie: Sbornik statei [Mediation: Act: Development: Compiled works]. Izhevsk: Publ. ERGO, 2010. 279 p. (In Russ.)
- Elkonin B.D. Intermediary Action and Development. Kul'turno-istoricheskaya psikhologiya = Cultural-Historical Psychology, 2016. Vol. 12, no. 3, pp. 93—112. (In Russ.) DOI:10.17759/chp.2016120306
- El'konin D.B. Izbrannye psikhologicheskie trudy [Selected psychological works]. Moscow: Publ. Pedagogika, 1989. 560 p. (In Russ.)
- Elkoninova L.I. Unity of Affect and Intellect in Socio-Dramatic Play. Kul'turno-istoricheskaya psikhologiya = Cultural-Historical Psychology, 2016. Vol. 12, no. 3, pp. 247—254. (In Russ.) DOI:10.17759/chp.2016120314
Information About the Authors
Metrics
Web Views
Whole time: 61
Previous month: 0
Current month: 61
PDF Downloads
Whole time: 10
Previous month: 0
Current month: 10
Total
Whole time: 71
Previous month: 0
Current month: 71