The Relationship of the Attitude to Cheating with Social Beliefs and the Supposed Attitude of Others in High School Students

58

Abstract

Academic dishonesty is a widespread but dangerous phenomenon, as it forms tolerance for fraud in other areas, including through tolerance of the dishonesty of others. The article is devoted to the analysis of the correlation between the subjective assessment of the permissibility of academic dishonesty and belief in a competitive world (BCW), the supposed assessment of peers and adults among schoolchildren, loyal and not loyal to dishonesty. It is considered both the attitude towards cheating itself (active dishonesty), as well as the message about the dishonesty of others and the refusal to help in dishonesty (passive dishonesty). 507 people were recruited for the research, 296 of them girls, aged from 13 to 18 years, average age of 15,6 (±1,38), who completed the questionnaire online. The level of BCW was measured using the questionnaire "Scale of belief in a competitive world, short version" by J. Dakkit adapted by O.A. Gulevich and colleagues. To assess the permissibility of cheating, were used vignettes, which described dishonest behavior and judgments about this behavior. Participants were supposed to assess them on a Likert scale from 1 to 9. Each type of dishonesty (cheating itself, reporting cheating and refusing to help with cheating) was represented by 3 vignettes. The results showed that the assessments of the admissibility of certain aspects of cheating are not related to each other and have a different structure of links with the BCW and the intended assessment of others. The results confirm the data on the greater complexity of honesty compared to dishonesty. For the disloyal, the permissibility of cheating is associated with more factors than for the loyal. Active dishonesty in loyal people is associated with prevalence, in disloyal people – with BCW and the opinion of parents.

General Information

Keywords: academic dishonesty; high and low loyalty to cheating; perceived norms; belief in a competitive world

Journal rubric: Developmental Psychology

Article type: scientific article

DOI: https://doi.org/10.17759/pse.2024290302

Received: 01.02.2024

Accepted:

For citation: Ulybina E.V., Tokareva A.A. The Relationship of the Attitude to Cheating with Social Beliefs and the Supposed Attitude of Others in High School Students. Psikhologicheskaya nauka i obrazovanie = Psychological Science and Education, 2024. Vol. 29, no. 3, pp. 20–30. DOI: 10.17759/pse.2024290302.

Full text

Introduction

Academic dishonesty (cheating) is an unpleasant phenomenon, but widespread [1; 3; 15; 18], it manifests itself in various forms, but in all cases it involves violating academic rules for the sake of some benefits. Cheating lays down so-called "neutralizing attitudes" [14], which contribute to the fact that dishonesty becomes possible, since it is normal [19] and as a result it is reproduced in the workplace [17].

The attitude to dishonesty includes both an assessment of the permissibility of their own dishonest behavior (direct dishonesty), and an assessment of the message that someone is cheating and agreeing to help the cheater (indirect dishonesty). The subject of the research in the work is the connection between the assessment of the permissibility of direct and indirect cheating with general ideas about whether there are rules in the world and how they work, about its prevalence, the expected reaction of classmates, teachers and parents among high school students who are loyal and not loyal to dishonesty.

Belief in a competitive world (BCW), in accordance with the theory of J. Dakkitta [7] is based on the belief that there are no rules, the world is built on the agreement of everyone with everyone and everything is possible to achieve goals. In such a world, dishonesty is not only acceptable, but also desirable. Research shows that BCW is directly related to self-esteem of dishonesty [5], willingness to cheat at a job interview [9] and corrupt intentions [26]. Although the contribution of BCW to the assessment of the permissibility of cheating among schoolchildren has not been sufficiently studied, it can be assumed that in the perception of a world in which there are no rules, cheating will be considered acceptable.

Both declarative and perceived norms contribute to the assessment of the permissibility of a particular behavior. Declarative ones are fixed in official rules, and perceived ones are a subjective idea of what is possible and what is not possible in the current social reality, what will be approved or not approved [16]. In cases where perceived norms do not coincide with declarative ones, behavior is determined by the influence of perceived ones, since their processing for decision-making is based on heuristic (obvious) rather than systematic information processing, which requires less cognitive effort [13].

Research shows that the assessment of the prevalence of cheating is one of the most significant predictors of dishonesty among students and schoolchildren [6; 10; 15; 18; 30]. The strong influence of peer behavior may indicate that academic dishonesty is not only learned by observing peer behavior, but also that peer behavior provides a kind of normative support for deception [15].

Perceived norms are formed both from a subjective assessment of the extent of dishonesty, and from the expected reaction of others, which for schoolchildren are classmates, teachers, parents. The likelihood of cheating increases in a situation where a person is sure that others, especially classmates, will not report the violation, and, if necessary, will help to deceive, and adults will not condemn or punish.

The willingness of students to report rule violations is considered one of the significant factors in reducing dishonesty [22], but research shows that it is not common and is considered very undesirable behavior [3; 22; 23; 27; 29]. Anyone who decides to report violations on the part of a friend risks being rejected by the team [22].

Another important component of the attitude towards cheating is assistance in fraud. In this case, the assistant also behaves dishonestly, but does it for the sake of another, which can be perceived as morally acceptable behavior [28]. However, the attitude towards the decision to help or refuse dishonesty and the relationship of the decision with the assessment of the admissibility of active dishonesty and other factors have not been sufficiently investigated.

The real disapproval of cheating on the part of classmates and classmates is one of the most significant factors in preventing dishonesty [12; 15; 30]. If many students have a negative attitude towards cheating, then it is difficult to get help with cheating, and there is a high probability that someone informs a teacher [12].

Although the opinion of adults is in many cases less important for high school students than the opinion of their peers, studies, according to the results of a meta-analysis by G. Tabares and colleagues [24] show a significant contribution of the position of parents to the prosocial behavior of adolescents. This allows us to assume that parents' opinions about various aspects of cheating are related to the assessment of the permissibility of such behavior.

The position of teachers also plays an important role. Cheating among schoolchildren is significantly inversely related to the disapproval of teachers [1], and the willingness to report dishonesty is directly related to teachers' support for such behavior [23].

However, research shows that in the same conditions, people cheat to varying degrees. And, although the level of deception in laboratory experiments is directly related to the general honesty index in the country [8], people who are influenced by common perceived norms differ in their tendency to deceive [11; 21]. Unlike most traits, the levels of dishonesty do not have a normal distribution, and depending on the tasks, either a small part of the participants lie to the maximum extent, or a small part lies minimally [11]. It is relevant to identify the psychological differences between "liars" and "non-liars". For example, students who are loyal and disloyal to dishonesty have a different structure of connection with faith in a just world and the possible results of dishonesty [4].

This suggests that the permissibility of dishonesty among loyal and non-loyal schoolchildren also has a different structure of connections with the idea of the existence of rules and norms in the world, which is reflected in the BCW, with the prevalence of cheating and the expected reactions of others.

The study was organized to test the following hypotheses:

  1. Students loyal to dishonesty rate the prevalence of direct and indirect cheating significantly higher, and the negative reaction of classmates, teachers and parents significantly less than disloyal ones.
  2. Those loyal to dishonesty consider both direct and indirect cheating less acceptable than those who are not loyal, the reaction of classmates is more negative, and adults are more positive.
  3. Students who are loyal to dishonesty have significantly higher BCW than those who are disloyal.
  4. Direct and indirect forms of dishonesty are directly related to both loyal and disloyal dishonesty.
  5. The permissibility of all forms of dishonesty among those loyal to cheating is directly related to the BCW, the prevalence assessment, and vice versa – with the alleged negative reaction to the dishonesty of classmates.
  6. The permissibility of dishonesty among those who are disloyal to cheating is directly related to BCW and the alleged negative reaction of adults.

Empirical Research

Sampling and methods

The study participants were 507 people, 296 of them girls. Age from 13 to 18 years, average 15.7. The differences in age are not significant.

The level of BCW was measured using the questionnaire "Scale of faith in the competitive world, short version" [2], consisting of 12 statements. Examples: "Money and wealth are exactly what is valuable in life," "A person who has an advantage in a situation should use it in any way to achieve his goal." Agreement with the statements is assessed on a 5-point Likert scale.

3 vignettes were used in each case to analyze attitudes towards the permissibility of cheating, reporting cheating to the teacher and refusing to help with cheating.

After each vignette, participants were asked to rate agreement with the following statements on a 9-point Likert scale.

  1. This is a common behavior.
  2. If classmates find out about it, they will treat him worse.
  3. He will have disagreements and misunderstandings with teachers or the administration, which may develop into a conflict.
  4. If parents find out about this, they will most likely disapprove of his actions.

The analysis of the results showed that the Cronbach's alpha for agreeing with the statements for all vignettes is 0.795, which indicates a fairly high consistency of responses and makes it possible to consider the total value as an indicator of the admissibility of dishonesty. Gender differences are not significant in all cases, which made it possible to consider the sample as a whole, without dividing subgroups of girls and boys.

Statement of results

To identify subsamples with high and low levels of cheating tolerance, the upper and lower quartiles of the distribution of the agreement were used with the statement that it is possible to do so. The subsample with a low tolerance of cheating consisted of 127 respondents with scores from 3 to 9 points (the lower quartile of the general distribution), the subsample with a high tolerance included 126 respondents with scores from 18 to 27 points. Since in most cases the distribution in the subsamples according to the Shapiro-Wilk criterion differed from the normal one, non-parametric criteria were subsequently used.

The analysis of the results showed that the permissibility of dishonesty has no significant links with the permissibility of reporting the dishonesty of others and with the permissibility of refusing to participate in dishonesty for either loyal or disloyal to dishonesty, which suggests the absence of a holistic attitude of schoolchildren to cheating.

The Mann-Whitney criterion was used to test the hypothesis of differences in agreement with judgments and the level of BCM. The results are shown in Table 1.

Table 1. Differences in agreement with statements and the level of social beliefs among students who are loyal and disloyal to dishonesty

 

Disloyal

to dishonesty

Loyal

to dishonesty

 

 

 

Mean

Std.Dev.

Skewness

Kurtosis

Mean

Std.Dev.

Skewness

Kurtosis

U

Effect size

The permissibility

of dishonesty

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Prevalence

13,323

4,963

0,033

-0,443

20,738

4,400

-0.367

-0,595

2208,0****

0,724

Deterioration of classmates' relationships

16,283

3,390

0,341

2,630

14,151

3,816

-1,030

2,450

5871,0***

0,266

Problems

with teachers

17,929

5,790

-0,310

-0,439

15,794

6,660

-0,111

-0,811

6507,5

0,187

Disapproval

of parents

20,118

5,570

-0,689

-0,174

14,929

6,454

-0,124

-0,779

4342,0****

0,457

The message

of dishonesty

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Prevalence

10,780

6,309

0,770

-0,010

11,841

7,442

0,550

-0,762

7533,5

0,058

Acceptable

10,142

6,934

0,810

-0,212

8,563

6,357

1,070

0,390

6874,5

0,141

Deterioration of classmates' relationships

21,370

5,425

-1,17

1,57

22,183

5,262

-0,853

-0,207

7188,0

0,102

Problems

with teachers

11,024

6,889

0,634

-0,362

12,262

7,118

0,408

-0,515

7220,5

0,098

Disapproval

of parents

14,906

6,592

-0,022

-0,546

17,214

6,252

-0,253

-0,294

6349,5*

0,206

Refusal to help with dishonesty

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

The prevalence of failure

15,646

6,060

-0,075

-0,497

16,397

6,433

-0,057

-0,611

7454,0

0,068

Acceptable

16,685

6,234

-0,183

-0,560

14,627

6,416

0,121

-0,648

6502,0

0,187

Deterioration of classmates' relationships

18,031

4,381

-0,306

1,540

19,651

4,733

-0,149

-0,276

6474,5

0,191

Problems

with teachers

10,055

5,096

0,318

-0,541

12,127

7,011

0,338

-0,734

6693,5

0,163

Disapproval

of parents

11,614

5,326

-0,036

-0,754

14,175

5,894

0,141

-0,114

6089,5***

0,230

BCW

44,276

15,258

-0,008

-0,489

59,833

13,133

-0,061

0,811

3433,5****

0,571

Note: * - <0,05, ** - <0,01, *** < 0,001, ****< 0,0001 The significance is given taking into account the Bonferroni correction.

 

Spearman's criterion was used to test the hypothesis of the correlation between assessing the admissibility of dishonesty, reporting the dishonesty of others and refusing to participate in dishonesty with social beliefs and the alleged attitude of others. The results are presented in Table 2.

Table 2. The correlation of assessing the acceptability of dishonesty, reporting the dishonesty of others and refusing to participate in dishonesty with the perceived attitude of others and social beliefs

 

Disloyal to dishonesty

Loyal to dishonesty

 

Dishonesty

Message

Refusal

Dishonesty

Message

Refusal

Prevalence

0,109

0,464****

0,466****

0,414****

0,481****

0,260

Deterioration

of classmates' relationships

-0,039

-0,311***

-0,046

-0,166

-0,680****

-0,194

Problems

with teachers

-0,093

-0,035

-0,335***

-0,01

-0,369****

-0,115

Disapproval

of parents

-0,381***

-0,207

-0,319***

0,089

-0,099

-0,031

BCW

0,290**

-0,023

-0,338****

0,025

-0,098

-0,092

Note: * - <0,05, ** - <0,01, *** < 0,001, ****< 0,0001 The significance is given taking into account the Bonferroni correction.

Discussion

The results of comparing agreement with judgments about cheating options suggest that loyal and non-loyal students of the same school have different perceived norms of direct dishonesty, and that for loyal students, dishonesty is more consistent with perceived norms and the assumed opinion of peers.  These differences can be explained by the effect of false agreement [20], according to which people tend to attribute their vision of the world to the majority. And the difference in parents' reactions is most likely due to real differences in family upbringing.

However, the idea of the prevalence of indirect dishonesty and the reaction of others to it among those loyal and not loyal to cheating are similar. Indirect dishonesty is carried out in a social context and its perception can be more objective.

As expected, the permissibility of all aspects of cheating has a different structure of connection with BCW and the expected reaction of others. The rejection of dishonesty, as a more complex behavior among those who are not loyal to cheating, is not related to the reproduction of what can be directly observed, but to the norms learned from parents and the general idea of how the world works. Research [25] shows that honesty requires greater cognitive resources, and schoolchildren need more grounds for honest behavior, both the general low belief that not all means are good, and the opinion of parents. In the absence of such restraining factors, students act primarily on the basis of a direct impulse, relying solely on perceived norms.

The links of the permissibility of indirect dishonesty, which presupposes social interaction, are structured differently.

The structure of the links between the permissibility of reporting cheating is similar for both loyal and disloyal. Schoolchildren are guided only by perceived norms, and loyal ones are also guided by the opinion of teachers. This corresponds to the data of Stevenson and colleagues on the dependence of the probability of reporting dishonesty on the behavior of teachers [23]. The connection with the BCW and the opinion of the parents is not significant in any case. The fact that it is impossible to "surrender" others is supported by social relations and is not related to ideas about the structure of the world.

For those who are disloyal to dishonesty, refusal to help is associated with almost all the factors considered, except for the reaction of classmates. Perhaps this is a rather difficult decision for them, affecting both the general idea of rules and social norms. And for loyal people, no connection is significant, probably the decision is made without taking into account the factors considered, for example, on the basis of sympathy for those asking or their own benefits.

Conclusions

The data obtained are consistent with the idea that honesty is more complex than dishonesty, clarifying the differences in the nature of the correlation between active and passive dishonesty with perceived norms and one's own values.

Direct dishonesty (self-cheating) among the non-loyal is related to the general idea of the world, the value system and the opinion of parents and is not related to perceived norms. And for loyal people, the permissibility of their own dishonesty is related to the behavior and reaction of others, but not to the BCW and parental attitudes.

The correlation of indirect forms of cheating with other factors is differentiated in both loyal and disloyal people. Reporting the dishonesty of others, as a condemned action, is associated with perceived norms for everyone. There are probably no uniform standards for helping others to be dishonest. And for the loyal, it is probably related to factors that were not taken into account in the study, and for the non-loyal, the decision to help is related to all factors except the opinion of the teachers.

Perhaps the reasons for the different attitudes towards dishonesty in a single environment are determined by the difference in family upbringing, which needs special study, as well as the possibility of forming other collective norms.

References

  1. Gizhitskii V.V. Uchebnyi obman kak strategiya psevdoadaptivnogo povedeniya u starsheklassnikov [Educational deception as a strategy of pseudo-adaptive behavior in high school students]. Uchenye zapiski Orlovskogo gosudarstvennogo universiteta. Seriya: Gumanitarnye i sotsial’nye nauki [Scientific notes of the Oryol State University. Series: Humanities and social sciences], 2014, no. 2(58), pp. 293–299. (In Russ.).
  2. Gulevich O.A., Anikeenok O.A., Bezmenova I.K. Sotsial'nye verovaniya: adaptatsiya metodik Dzh. Dakkita [Social beliefs: adapting the methods of J. Bakkit]. Psikhologiya. Zhurnal Vysshei shkoly ekonomiki [Psychology. Journal of the Higher School of Economics], 2014, no. 11(2), pp. 68–89. DOI:10.17323/1813-8918-2014-2-68-89 (In Russ.).
  3. Latova N.V., Latov Yu.V. Obman v uchebnom protsesse [Deception in the educational process]. Obshchestvennye nauki i sovremennost' [Social sciences and modernity], 2007, no. 1, pp. 31–46. DOI:10.21626/innova/2018.3/02 (In Russ.).
  4. Ulybina E.V., Tokareva A.A. Svyaz' very v spravedlivyi mir s otnosheniem k akademicheskoi nechestnosti u shkol'nikov s vysokoi i nizkoi loyal'nost'yu k chiterstvu [The connection of belief in a just world with the attitude to academic dishonesty among schoolchildren with high and low loyalty to cheating]. Psikhologicheskaya nauka i obrazovanie [Psychological Science and Education], 2022, no. 27(4), pp. 59–67. DOI:10.17759/pse.2022270406 (In Russ.).
  5. Cai W., Wu S. Greater death anxiety, greater dishonesty for self-benefit: The moderating role of social dominance orientation. Motivation and Emotion, 2021. Vol. 45, pp. 368–376. DOI:10.1007/s11031-021-09877-3
  6. Daumiller M., Janke S. Effects of performance goals and social norms on academic dishonesty in a test. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 2020. Vol. 90(2), pp. 537–559. DOI:10.31234/osf.io/c8uzk
  7. Duckitt J., Wagner C., du Plessis I. The psychological bases of ideology and prejudice: testing a dual process model. Journal of personality and social psychology, 2002. Vol. 83(1), pp. 75–93. DOI:10.1037/0022-3514.83.1.75
  8. Gächter S., Schulz J.F. Intrinsic honesty and the prevalence of rule violations across societies. Nature, 2016. Vol. 7595, pp. 496–499. DOI:10.1038/nature17160
  9. Ho J.L., Powell D.M., Spence J.R. Willingness to fake: Examining the impact of competitive climate and hiring situations. International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 2020. Vol. 28(3), pp. 247–263. DOI:10.1111/ijsa.12288
  10. Ives B., Giukin L. Patterns and predictors of academic dishonesty in Moldovan university students. Journal of Academic Ethics, 2020. Vol. 18, pp. 71–88. DOI:10.1007/s10805-019-09347-z
  11. Jiang Q., Zhang Y., Zhu Z. Is dishonesty normally distributed? Evidence from six behavioral experiments and a simulation study. Personality and Individual Differences, 2023. Vol. 205, P. 112105. DOI:10.1016/j.paid.2023.112105
  12. Magnus J.R., Polterovich V.M., Danilov D.L. Tolerance of cheating: An analysis across countries. The Journal of Economic Education, 2002. Vol. 33(2), pp. 125–135. DOI:10.1080/00220480209596462
  13. Manning M. The effects of subjective norms on behaviour in the theory of planned behaviour: A meta‐analysis. British journal of social psychology, 2019. Vol. 48(4), pp. 649–705. DOI:10.1348/014466608X393136
  14. Matza D., Sykes G. Techniques of neutralization: A theory of delinquency. American Sociological Review, 1957. Vol. 22(6), pp. 664–670. DOI:10.2307/2089195
  15. McCabe D.L., Treviño L.K., Butterfield K.D. Cheating in academic institutions: A decade of research. Ethics & Behavior, 2001. Vol. 11(3), pp. 219–232. DOI:10.1207/S15327019EB1103_2
  16. Morris M.W., Hong Y.Y., Chiu C.Y. Normology: Integrating insights about social norms to understand cultural dynamics. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 2015. Vol. 129, pp. 1–13. DOI:10.1016/j.obhdp.2015.03.001
  17. Mulisa F., Ebessa A.D. The carryover effects of college dishonesty on the professional workplace dishonest behaviors: A systematic review. Cogent Education, 2021. Vol. 8(1), p. 1935408. DOI:10.1080/2331186X.2021.1935408
  18. Murdock T.B., Anderman E.M. Motivational perspectives on student cheating: Toward an integrated model of academic dishonesty. Educational psychologist, 2006. Vol. 41(3), pp. 129–145. DOI:10.1207/s15326985ep4103_1
  19. Murdock T.B., Stephens J.M. Is cheating wrong? Students' reasoning about academic dishonesty. Psychology of academic cheating. Academic Press, 2007, pp. 229–251. DOI:10.1016/B978-012372541-7/50014-0
  20. Ross L., Greene D., House P. The “false consensus effect”: An egocentric bias in social perception and attribution processes. Journal of experimental social psychology, 1977. Vol. 13(3), pp. 279–301. DOI:10.1016/0022-1031(77)90049-X
  21. Serota K.B., Levine T.R., Zvi L. The ubiquity of long-tail lie distributions: seven studies from five continents. Journal of Communication, 2023, p. jqad040. DOI:10.1093/joc/jqad040
  22. Simon C.A., Carr J.R., McCullough S.M. Gender, student perceptions, institutional commitments and academic dishonesty: Who reports in academic dishonesty cases. Assessment & Evaluation in Higher Education, 2004. Vol. 29(1), pp. 75–90. DOI:10.1080/0260293032000158171
  23. Stevenson S.M., Flannigan K., Willey A. Exploring Factors That Contribute to Nursing Students’ Willingness to Report Peer Academic Integrity Violations. Nursing Education Perspectives, 2023. Vol. 44(3), pp. 140–146. DOI:10.1097/01.NEP.0000000000001090
  24. Tabares A.S.G., Arenas D.A.M., Duque M.C.C. Research trends on the relationship between parenting and prosocial behaviors in children and adolescents. Revista Interamericana de Psicología/Interamerican Journal of Psychology, 2023. Vol. 57(1), pp. e1378–e1378. DOI:10.30849/ripijp.v57i1.1378
  25. Van’t Veer A.E., Stel M., van Beest I. Limited capacity to lie: Cognitive load interferes with being dishonest. Judgment and Decision Making, 2014. Vol. 9(3), pp. 199–206. DOI:10.1017/S1930297500005751
  26. Vilanova F., Milfont T.L., Costa A.B. A dual process social psychological model of corrupt intention and attitudes toward corrupt people. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2022. Vol. 123(4), pp. 854–883. DOI:10.1037/pspp0000414
  27. Waltzer T., Samuelson A., Dahl A. Students’ reasoning about whether to report when others cheat: Conflict, confusion, and consequences. Journal of Academic Ethics, 2021, pp. 1–23. DOI:10.1007/s10805-021-09414-4
  28. Wu D., Loke Xu F., Lee K. Neural correlates of evaluations of lying and truth-telling in different social contexts. Brain research, 2011. Vol. 1389, pp. 115–124. DOI:10.1016/j.brainres.2011.02.084
  29. Yachison S., Okoshken J., Talwar V. Students’ reactions to a peer’s cheating behavior. Journal of Educational Psychology, 2018. Vol. 110(6), pp. 747–763. DOI:10.1037/edu0000227
  30. Zhao L., Mao H., Compton B.J. Academic dishonesty and its relations to peer cheating and culture: A meta-analysis of the perceived peer cheating effect. Educational Research Review, 2022. Vol. 36, p. 100455. DOI:10.1016/j.edurev.2022.100455

Information About the Authors

Elena V. Ulybina, Doctor of Psychology, Professor of the Department of General Psychology, Russian Presidential Academy of National Economy and Public Administration under the President of the Russian Federation, Moscow, Russia, ORCID: https://orcid.org/0000-0002-5398-9006, e-mail: evulbn@gmail.com

Aleksandra A. Tokareva, PhD Student, Russian Presidential Academy of National Economy and Public Administration under the President of the Russian Federation, Moscow, Russia, ORCID: https://orcid.org/0000-0002-3183-357X, e-mail: tokareva_aa@bk.ru

Metrics

Views

Total: 160
Previous month: 34
Current month: 24

Downloads

Total: 58
Previous month: 12
Current month: 13