Modern Problems of Children's Play: Cultural-Historical Context

797

Abstract

The purpose of this article is to analyze the state of modern research on children’s play, approaches to its study, as well as existing methods of its evaluation. The relevance of the topic is due to the leading role of the play in preschool childhood and the complexity of this phenomenon. Play is actively studied, and play interventions are often used. However, the analysis of the literature shows confusion and uncertainty of terminology due to a large spread of theoretical positions and methodological approaches to the study of play. This creates great difficulties in planning and conducting research, and affects their results. The article deals with the issues of defining and classifying play, understanding its structure and development. The main trends of modern research and their connection with classical game theories, the role of cultural-historical approach and the contribution of E.O. Smirnova to the study of play are shown.

General Information

Keywords: preschool, children’s play, pretend play, development of play, structure of play, play theory, cultural-historical approach, playful learning, methods of play evaluation

Journal rubric: Problems of Cultural-Historical and Activity Psychology

Article type: scientific article

DOI: https://doi.org/10.17759/chp.2020160307

Funding. This research was funded by Russian Science Foundation grant number 20-18-00423.

For citation: Veraksa N.E., Veresov N.N., Veraksa A.N., Sukhikh V.L. Modern Problems of Children's Play: Cultural-Historical Context. Kul'turno-istoricheskaya psikhologiya = Cultural-Historical Psychology, 2020. Vol. 16, no. 3, pp. 60–70. DOI: 10.17759/chp.2020160307.

Full text

 

 

Introduction

Dedicated to the memory of E.O. Smirnova

Scientific interest in play is more than one hundred and fifty years old. However, both recently and decades ago, authors are forced to state that play is notoriously difficult to define, always “escaping” from researchers [18; 27; 29; 35; 44; 50; 55; 62]. The variety of forms, types, and hypostases of play is too vast.

The majority of modern authors approach the problem of play definition from the point of view of revealing and describing every specific feature of play (criterion­based definition). Thus, play is most often defined as being voluntary, internally motivated, process-oriented rather than result-based, spontaneous, joyous and pleasant, active involvement with an occasional element of make-believe [21; 35; 44; 62]. Other authors [46; 51], based on L. Wittgenstein’s philosophical concept of “family resemblances”, suggest studying play without defining what play is or what it should be.

Despite its clearly ambiguous terminology, play is being actively studied — both in terms of theoretical conceptualization and in terms of collecting more and more new empirical data. At the same time, serious methodological problems are pointed out in empirical studies of play [13; 19; 35; 55; 58]. It is not always clear which aspect of play was in focus and why is it that different types and components of play are measured in many unrelated ways. The very methods of play evaluation are developed within the
framework of different theoretical approaches to its understanding, and accordingly, the attention of researchers is focused on different aspects of play, and they use different approaches to interpret its outcomes. That is why the mechanisms of play influence on development remain unclear and the chances for reproducing successful interventions are limited [55; 58]. In this regard, highly relevant is the analytical review of psychological papers that would highlight key contemporary problems of children’s play: approaches in defining, classifying, understanding the structure and development of play, the methodology of contemporary research, and methods it uses to assess play. Moreover, this analysis is important from a historical perspective, since classical game theories and child development and particularly the cultural-historical approach, continue to guide contemporary research in this field on a grand scale. No such overview is currently available in the Russian or foreign literature.

Classic theories of play

For more than one hundred years, researchers of play have been trying to define the role of play in children’s development which they see as an important factor in a child’s emotional, social and cognitive development. Table 1 summarizes some of the basic play theories in a historical perspective.

In the first half of the 20th century, two traditions began to develop that define the theoretical foundations of most modern play research. One of them is based on the theory of J. Piaget, who considers play in the context of cognitive development. Symbolic play is a form of assimilation; it interferes with accommodation and is a maladaptive process that children grow out of with time [33; 45]. For G. Piaget, play is an indicator of development rather than its engine. Its emergence in children of about 18 months signifies the development of the semiotic function — an ability to represent an absent object or an event that is not directly perceived through symbols or signs. Paradoxically, J. Piaget’s impact on researchers who held play to be important for development was enormous, although he himself did not consider play critical for the development of logical thinking which he studied.

If in the spirit of constructivism, J. Piaget believed the child to be a creator of its own cognition, L.S. Vygotsky, the founder of the other tradition, showed that cognitive development in play is primarily due to interaction between the child and a sensitive adult and other children [2]. Because children are capable of imagining something only from their own available experience, role play is predetermined by what is in culture. That is, children play at activities and roles that exist in a given culture.

Table 1

Main theories of play

Theory Title

A brief summary of the theory

1

2

Early theories [38; 50; 56]:

G. Sprncrs’s theory of surplus energy (1878)

Play is an uncontrollable drsire, a way to “let off stram”, to spend rxcrss energy in childhood.

M. Lazarus’s Theory of Relaxation (1883)

Play strms from a lack of energy and is nrrdrd to restore strength after work.

Theory of Exercise by K. Groos(1899)

In a broad srnsr, play is thr training of instincts and skills that will br nrrdrd in future adult lifr. For example, playing parents trains parenting skills.

Theory of recapitulation by S. Hall (1886)

Ontogrnrsis repeats phylogrny, and in play thr child plays out thr drvrlopmrntal stages of thr human race: thr animal, thr savagr, a srpsrsrntativr of traditional society, rtc. This hrlps thr child grt rid of primitive instincts that arc superfluous in modern society.

Pedagogical theory of play by J. Drwry (1900)

Teaching should br reduced mainly to play and labor activity, where rach action of thr child brcomrs an instrument of his knowledge, his own discovery, a way of comprehending thr truth. By reconstructing its experience in play, thr child prrcrivrs thr meaning of it and drvrlops consciousness and skills.

Classical theories:

Thr Psychodynamic Theory of Play by Z. Freud (1920)

Play is necessary for childsrn’s rmotional drvrlopmrnt. In play children can livr out thr fulfillment of their wishes, copr with traumatic experiences and strong nrgativr frrlings [38; 50; 56].

L.S. Vygotsky’s Cultural and Historical Approach (1933)

Play is a culturally conditioned phenomenon at thr samr timr it is “imburd” with thr child’s personal meaning and imagination. Peer play is of special importance. It is a transitional stagr from a child’s thinking, limited by thr properties of thr current situation, to thr thinking that is complrtrly free from thrsr limitations [2].

Cultural Theory by J. Huizinga (1938)

Human culture occurs and unfolds in play. Play is older than culture, brcausr all thr main features of play can also br observed in animals. Every gamr has certain rules, performs certain functions and brings plrasurr and joy [38; 50; 56].

The theory of socialization by G.H. Mrad (1934)

Play is a modrl of social interaction and a mrans of assimilating social attitudes, and thus is a mrans of formation of a socialized prrsonality. Play rolr is thr equivalent of a social rolr. Initial social attitudes arisr in frrr rolr-playing with changing rolrs, morr complex and grnrralizrd — in a gamr that has rulrs dur to thr fact that it is nrcrssary to takr into account simultanrously different rolr positions [4].



Classifications of play

In considering a child’s natural (without an adult) play as a basis for the child’s psychological well-being, B. Hughes describes 16 types of play: communication play, creative play, deep play, fantasy and imaginative play, dramatic play, exploratory play, locomotor play, object play, mastery play, recapitulative play, role play, rough and tumble play, social play, social-dramatic play and symbolic play [15]. This totally descriptive typology contains the names of games generally accepted in foreign literature, and also shows how unlike it is, for example, compared to the domestic classification, which is built on different foundations: directorial, figurative, storyline and role play, play with rules and play according to rules [5].

Games are also classified according to the degree of the child’s socialization in play. Development is considered to evolve from solitary play (a child plays by itself with objects/toys) to parallel play (children play nearby with similar objects/toys, i.e., share their space playing a solitary game) and associative play (children interact with each other by exchanging play materials). The highest level of socialization in play is cooperative play that occurs between two or more children when they start exchanging ideas about play and toys. Rules appear in such a game and everyone knows what role he or she is playing. The key difference from the previous stages is the emergence of communication about play itself [54].

Researchers pay special attention to pretend play. The classics believed this particular type of play to be dominant in preschool age and, in general, never considered the phenomenon of play in the perspective of a lifetime, as they held that play is important for development only in preschool childhood. In foreign literature, pretend play is an umbrella term for variants options of games that include an element of make-believe, acting, or “as if” presentations: dramatic play, role play, fantasy play, and playing with substitute objects [33; 62]. At the same time, play may be solitary or social pretend play and may partially combine with other types of play. For example, locomotor play combines with pretend play if children pretend to be fighters in the ring [62].

Play structure

Consideration of different theoretical approaches to play shows that they focus on different aspects of pretend play: affect, interaction between players and an adult, treatment of objects / toys, the degree of role acceptance, the content of play, the themes and complexity of stories, the nature of play actions, the system of rules and ways of play organization, the frequency and duration of games, etc.

L.S. Vygotsky believed that pretend and role-play had three key characteristics: children create an imaginary situation, assume roles and act upon them, and also follow a set of rules dictated by their specific roles. Each of these aspects is important and contributes to the development of higher mental functions [2].

D.B. Elkonin made a distinction between the theme of play (the sphere of reality that is reflected in the game) and its content (what from this sphere is precisely reflected in play). To describe the levels of play development he used four parameters: the content of play, roles, character and logic of play actions, and also the child’s reaction to the breach of play logic [16].

B. Thompson and T. Goldstein attempted to formulate the hierarchical model of play based on the analysis of almost two hundred modern articles on the relationship between play and child development. They identified the following stages/components of play: (1) object substitution (using an object as if it were not what it really is), (2) attributing imaginary properties/animation (attributing the properties of a living thing to an object that does not have them), (3) social interactions within a pretend act (two or more children agree to replace an object or attribute imaginary properties verbally or non­verbally), (4) role acceptance (the child pretends to be someone when interacting with other children) and (5) metacommunication involved in play (planning, agreements, rules, role distribution in order to organize role­play that includes complex scenarios and stories). In the authors’ opinion, it is sufficient for a child to demonstrate any of these components to enable him or her able to say that there is pretend play going on. In this case, more advanced components include all lower levels [58].

Play development

It is noted that when talking about the development of play most foreign researchers somehow reproduce the sequence described by J. Piaget: from sensorimotor to symbolic play and then to play with rules, the central element of which is symbolic representation and use of objects in an unusual, non-literal way. According to J. Piaget, typically developing children start to engage in this type of play before the age of 2 years and it reaches its climax when they are 3—4 years old and gradually fades away before the age of 6 years, although some children continue to play at an older age [42; 45].

In line with the cultural-historical approach, there are more meaningful concepts. One of them has been developed by E.E. Kravtsova and G.G. Kravtsov [5] and traces play development up to the adult age. The first kind of independent, “real” children’s play is directorial. It is still very similar to the object-manipulative activity, but already has all the features of play: dual-subjectivity (the child controls the progress of a game, but is also a participant in it), an imaginary situation (actions with objects gain meaning in its logic), toys and objects are used as means for plot realization. Then at the age of 3—4 there emerges imaginary play when a child identifies itself with someone or something and tries in its behavior to reproduce what it has identified itself with. There is neither pretend or role relations typical for pretend and role play which is due to emerge at the next stage — at the age of 4—5. This kind of play combines figurative and directorial lines of play development and mental development. If the previous two play types were mainly individual, then pretend and role play, even being realized by a single subject, is collective in its nature and implies obligatory interaction with other people, play partners. At the next stage of development comes play with rules (5—6 years) in which play actions are strictly subordinated to a concrete set of rules, and the rules define the character and features of the game itself. Appearance of play with rules coincides with the end of preschool age. Based on the two-subject criterion the authors, however, consider play development further: play according to rules (younger schoolchildren), literary play (teenage age), theatrical play (older teenage age), play with the image of “I” and playing at jobs (youth age) and, finally, adult games.

B. van Oers [42] offers his own view of play and its development in ontogenesis, based on A.N. Leontiev’s theory of activity. He believes that from the point of view of activity, play appears to be an absolutely cultural construct based on cultural traditions, practices and beliefs about how, when and why it can take place. In this case, play development is a process of self-regulation development through mastering more diverse and increasingly complex rules. It is the development of the ability to continue to play in increasingly complex and culturally regulated activities under the conditions of freedom admissible by established practice. Indeed, anthropological research has found pretend play to be present in all cultures. However, the frequency, themes, and parental involvement in these games vary from culture to culture. Family and social values are an important source of these differences [33; 46].

Contemporary play research

Over the past few decades, a large amount of data has been accumulated to support L.S. Vygotsky’s thesis about the key role of play in the development and education of preschool children [35]. Pretend play is believed to contribute to the development of social skills, creativity, intellect, theory of mind, executive functions, emotional regulation, counterfactual reasoning, symbolic thinking, and the ability to solve problems and provide arguments [1; 6; 22; 24; 28; 32; 34; 35; 39; 48; 53; 57]. There are also studies linking play and academic results [41; 59; 60]. Ait the same time, based on the outcomes of extensive research analysis, A. Lillard and colleagues had to state that if play was really related to a child’s development, it was still not obvious how critical this connection could be as there was a lot of evidence in favor of equifinality (play helps development, but it is only one of possible routes of development — other activities may work the same way or even better) and epiphenomenalism (play is an epiphe­nomenon or a by-product of another activity or condition that actually contributes to development) [35]. Much of this uncertainty is due to methodological difficulties in evaluating play and the imperfection of the tools that exist for this end. In addition to finding links between play and child development, contemporary research also examines play in the context of learning: how an adult can participate in play and use it for educational purposes.

Play and learning

All variations of approaches to the construction of “playful” learning are based on the cultural-historical theory of L, S. Vygotsky and the works of D.B. Elkonin and A.N. Leontiev. In particular, L.S. Vygotsky’s thesis that an adult (teacher, educator) is needed to expand the cultural and social experience of a child that could form the basis of his or her imagination and enrich the child’s play, acquaint the child with new forms of play and support its development [17; 43; 46; 47].

In foreign literature, there are a number of terms that reflect the use of play in the education of preschool children: playful learning, play-responsive learning, play­based learning. In general, play-based learning is a pedagogical approach that combines playful, child-initiated elements with the intervention of an adult who pursues some educational objectives [47]. Playful learning is an umbrella term that includes what is called both free and guided play [31]. Both types of play promote learning, but in guided play the adult pursues a certain educational objective and structures the gaming space accordingly. He can do this in two ways by simply providing suitable materials (e.g., paints to help them learn how to distinguish colors) or by joining the play. The adult’s participation in play, however, requires certain conditions to be met. In order not to ruin the child’s play, the adult should not restrict the child’s freedom or impose rules that contradict children’s playing needs at that moment [42; 43].

P. Hakkarainen identifies criteria for a successful adult intervention in children’s role-playing game: the idea of play should come from children while the adult actively participates in the discussion and helps to develop it; the adult grows into his role and plays too; the adult is emotionally involved in the game; he enters into spontaneous dialogues from the role and participates in the play events; he supports dramatic tension in the story, helps to develop a coherent and fascinating story; he promotes dynamics and involvement (for example, when the game might have become boring, the adult introduced a new character or a turn of events) [29].

Based on their interviews and observations in kindergarten groups, А. Pyle and E. Daniels identified five different play types that form a continuum in adult involvement: free play; assistance in a game where the teacher expands the children’s free play by making thematic “inputs” (e.g., bringing books about planes to children building an airplane); and collaborative play with a shared locus of control (the teacher and children together devise a context for a game, including the topic and necessary materials); play-based learning as an integration of skills that do not normally occur in play in a natural way (e.g., counting and recording the number of flowers in a flower store); didactic games designed to teach certain mandatory mathematical and linguistic elements [47].

М. Fleer also uses free play observations to formulate a typology that reflects a teacher’s actions and position in relation to the imagined situation: the teacher’s proximity to the game; the teacher’s intention is parallel to that of the children; the teacher follows the children’s game; the teacher is involved in conversations with the children about the imagined situation in their game; the teacher is inside the children’s game [37].

In the domestic tradition, the role of communication with an adult in the development of play was highlighted by M.I. Lisina: the emergence of role-playing is genetically associated with the formation of object actions under the guidance of adults in early childhood and with a change in the nature of communication, when a child begins to look for an adult’s attention and approval of his or her own play and object actions [3]. Е. О. Smirnova, a student of M.I. Lisina, introduced and analyzed the concept of “a teacher’s play competence”. She considered different variants of the educator’s position in children’s play: detached, didactic and supporting, all of them being based on play competence. A teacher with a detached position does not pay attention to children playing, taking a “let-them-play-as-they-want” attitude to it and regarding it a useless activity. When he assumes a didactic position, all the teacher’s activities are aimed at teaching children to play. Being aware of the importance of play for development, the teacher wants children to play correctly, so he himself allots the roles, provides the game plot, prompting them who is to say what, etc. Such an educator is characterized by confidence in his or her judgment and desire to pass their knowledge and experience to children. And if children deviate from a given course by showing initiative, it is perceived as a violation of the norm which has to be suppressed.

The teacher’s supportive attitude aims to back children’s initiative in a game. The teacher is both a partner in, and organizer of, a game. The degree and nature of his or her involvement depend on children’s situation and play skills. To show children examples of higher-level play, the teacher may address them on behalf of a new role, to suggest a new turn of the plot if the game gets “stuck”, and to take on an additional role that stimulates plot development.

Such a teacher plays along with children and his or her play competence consists of three main abilities: developed imagination that makes it possible to overcome аНтеои-уеа and create new images and plots; emotional expression and artistry that involve children in an imagined situation; and, finally, support for children’s own initiative and autonomy and confidence in their own abilities [8]. In doing so, E.O. Smirnova contrasts play as an independent aetiviiy and play-based learning tools. The use of pl^^^^b^^^d learning methods implies not only an adult’s initiative but also his or her direct guidance. For ext^trnple. games based on adult-developed scenarios, the use of toys or fairy-tale plots in class have nothing to do with real play and do not lead to the development of a child’s independence [7].

Play research methods

To assess preschoolers’ play activity, E.O. Smirnova developed a method that procedurally consists of observation in specially created conditions [6]. After an object environment has been modeled in the playroom, a group of 2-4 children are encouraged to play on their own. Observers assess a game which is free from adults’ suggestions or images that are embodied in toys. Among the materials offered to children, are the multifunc­tional, “open” materials: fabrics of different textures, a roll of fabric, clothespins, ropes, ribbons, ribbands, elastic bands, small logs and sticks, wooden rings, cup liners, chestnuts, cones, cardboard boxes of different sizes, etc. All these materials are placed within the children’s reach. On average, a group of children is observed for 40 minutes. The following blocks and indicators are assessed in conditional points from 0 (total absence) to 3 (a high degree of manifestation): substitution level (object, positional, spatial ones); interaction (organizational and intra-play); and the play plan (level, extent, execution and sustainability of the play idea).

Let us also consider several frequently mentioned and widely-used foreign methods [20; 23; 25; 48; 49; 55; 58].

Child-Initiated Pretend Play Assessment (ChiPPA) is an observation-based methodology that both assesses play in specially created conditions and excludes adult intervention. But unlike the previous method, it assesses solitary play. First, the child is offered toys with clear functionality (for example, miniature animal figures) and then unstructured materials such as pieces of fabric and sticks. The observation lasts for 18 or 30 minutes. The observers assess the level of complexity and self-organization in the game: the percentage of specific play actions, the number of object substitutions and the number of imitation actions. The tool was created for therapeutic practice and it also makes it possible to define play themes and styles that indicate possible deficits in the game.

The Affect in Play Scale — Preschool (APS-P) is another standardized observation-based tool that focuses on affective manifestations and cognitive components of play. The child is told a short unfinished story and then is asked to play on his or her own and simultaneously is provided with a set of plastic animals, cups, a car toy and a rubber ball. The session is recorded on video.

Same as in the previous method, solitary play alone is assessed here. The scale of cognitive assessment includes play organization (quality and complexity of the plot), imagination (novelty and uniqueness of play), comfort in play (engagement in, and pleasure from, play). The scale of affect estimation includes frequency of affective manifestations, variety of affective manifestations (from 11 affective manifestations: happiness/ satisfaction; anx- iety/fear; sadness/pain; frustration/dissatisfaction; care /bonding; aggression; oral aggression, etc.), the intensity of affective manifestations (on a scale of 1 to 5). As seen from the list of the indicators being assessed, the methodology is developed in line with the psychoanalytic approach in order to, first of all, provide information for planning therapeutic interventions and tracking their effectiveness. Therefore, here play serves rather as a context for assessing the psychological state of the child.

The Test of Pretend Play (ToPP; formerly known as the Warwick Symbolic Play Test or WSPT), unlike previous tools, is a structured test method that focuses on the symbolic aspects of play and offers verbal and nonverbal test options for children aged 1—6 years: with items from everyday life, with toys and unstructured materials, with toys only or without toys and materials.

During the test session, the tester invites a child to play and provides standardized hints if necessary. Three types of symbolic play are assessed: object substitution (for example, a napkin as a blanket), referring to a missing object as if it were there (for example, drinking imaginary tea) and assigning imaginary characteristics to an object (for example, the doll is sick). In addition, the child’s ability to link several symbolic actions into a meaningful sequence is assessed. Raw test scores can be translated into age norms. The theoretical basis of this test is also evident from its design and assessed parameters — the theory of J. Piaget and the psychology of development, where play is an indicator of the child’s level of cognitive development.

The Smilansky Scale for the Evaluation of Dramatic and Socio-Dramatic Play (SSEDSP) assesses peer play through in vivo observation of free play. The duration of the observation is 20 or 30 minutes, this time is divided into short intervals. Qualitative and quantitative assessment allows one to draw conclusions about the level of development of the storyline and pretend-role playing. The test assesses the presence or absence of six pretend play elements, four of which are typical for solitary play, two for peer-only play: imitation (figurative) pretend-role play, object substitution, make-believe when referring to actions and situations, duration of role play (at least, 10 minutes long), interaction (at least, two children interact in the context of a play episode) and verbal communication in play. The play environment should include materials associated with a household, a hospital, a store, unstructured materials, clothes for changing and a set of tools. The scale was originally designed to assess the development of play among children at risk level from low-income families.

The Penn Interactive Peer Play Scale (PIPPS) is a 32-point questionnaire with versions for parents and teachers. Just as the previous scale, it was designed to do research on children from low-income families. The teacher/parent should note how they observed a particular behavior in a child’s free play. Three parameters are evaluated on the Likert scale: disruption of the play process, quitting the game and active participation in play interaction. That is, it is not play itself that is evaluated, but the nature of interpersonal interaction with peers in the context of play.

From this brief description we can see how the understanding of play and the content of methodology depend on the purpose of its creation and the theoretical basis. Most methodologies focus on the child and not on play. Play serves as a means or context for diagnosing the child’s social adaptation, affective or cognitive spheres. Of the methodologies described in this article, only two are directly focused on the evaluation of play and its significant components, and both of them are developed within the cultural-historical approach, reflecting the definition of play given in the works by L.S. Vygotsky and D.B. Elko- nin (the Smilansky Scale for the Evaluation of Dramatic and Socio-Dramatic Play and E.O. Smirnova’s Method of Evaluating the Level of Play). Their comparative description is provided in Table 2, which shows that the national method allows for the most complete and differentiated assessment of the level of children’s play development. It is noteworthy, however, that none of the methods reflect how a child experiences play and play events [37] and record the theme and content of play [16].

Table 2

Comparison of play evaluation methods: The Smilansky Scale for the Evaluation of Dramatic and Socio-Dramatic Play and E.O. Smirnova’s Method of Assessment of the Play Activity Leve

Description of the method

The Smilansky Scale for the Evaluation of Dramatic and Socio-Dramatic Play

E.O. Smirnova’s Method of Assessment of the Play Activity Level

1

2

3

Procedure

Observation in the natural environment

Observation in specially created conditions

Object of observation

Free play — solitary and with peers

Peer play

Duration of observation

20 or 30 minutes long, divided into 5-minute intervals

40 minutes

Materials

Toys and role play attributes imitating real objects

Polyfunctional “open” materials

Scale of evaluation

The Likert Scale from 0 (total absence) to 3 (vivid degree of manifestation)

The Likert Scale from 0 (total absence) to 3 (vivid degree of manifestation)

Evaluation result

Level of pretend play development

Level of pretend play development

Evaluated Play Components according to Thompson & Goldstein, 2019

 

 

(1) Substitution of objects;

+

+

(2) Assignment of imaginary properties/animation;

 

+

(3) Social interactions within make-believe;

+ (actions and verbal communication in play)

+

(4) Role acceptance;

+ (image role play and make-believe about actions and situations)

+

(5) Metacommunication related to play organization;

 

+

 

+ duration of a play episode

+ Spatial substitution (creation and semantic differentiation of the play space)

 

 

+ evaluation of the play plan (level of the idea, its expansion, execution of the idea and sustainability of the play idea)

From the point of view of the application procedure the methods can be divided into several categories: observation in vivo; observation in laboratory (specially created) conditions; structured task-based methods; a questionnaire for adults from the child’s environment (teachers, parents). There are also methods that are based on self-reporting, but they are usually designed for older children and adults. An example of this method is the Fantasy Play Interview/Imaginative Play Predisposition Interview, where a child is asked about his or her favorite play, what he or she likes to do alone, about talking to himself or herself and thoughts before going to bed, etc. The interview assesses play orientation to find out if it is fantasy-oriented or reality-oriented [20].

Conclusions

1.     Play in childhood is an extremely complex phenomenon. It starts and becomes more complex in onto­genesis at a preschool age, it may take different forms and serve purposefully for developmental and educational goals. Play is actively and widely studied by using different methods including a formative experiment. The literature analysis demonstrates confusion and vagueness of the terminology due to a large scattering of theoretical views and methodological approaches to play research. This creates major methodological difficulties in the development and conduct of empirical research and influences the results of these studies, and, consequently, the ideas about the impact of play on a child’s life.

2.     The two classical views on preschool children’s play continue to significantly determine modern research trends in this area. According to the first point of view formulated in J. Piaget’s operational theory of intellectual development, play is seen as an indicator of development rather than its driving force. The followers of the second point of view, which is based on the cultural-historical concept of L.S. Vygotsky [1; 2; 5; 39; 53], describe play as a leading activity for preschool children in the course of which the child development takes place (L.S. Vygotsky, A.N. Leontiev, D.B. Elkonin, etc.).

3.     The scientific literature presents various play classifications. Their differences are determined by the interpretation of the phenomenon of play, which is based on the author’s theoretical positions, the age group of the participants in play, and other characteristics [5; 15; 18; 27; 29; 35; 44; 50; 55; 62]. The absence of a generally accepted classification and play theory indicates the relevance and necessity of research into this problem.

4.     Modern research presents two sets of data, some of which confirm the leading role of play in child development [22; 24; 28; 32; 34; 57]; others indicate a limited impact of play on child development [35].

5.     The publications reflect a strong trend towards a search for ways to use the play format in preschool education [43; 46; 47]. At the same time, it is noted that an educator can take different positions ranging from that of an active participant and leader of children’s play to that of a neutral observer [26].

6.     There is a variety of scales for assessing the level of play development. Most of them focus on the assessment of the child rather than that of play itself [20; 23; 25; 48; 49; 55; 58]. An informative assessment of a child’s play is made possible by using the scales based on L.S. Vy­gotsky’s cultural-historical theory of [6].

7.     We believe that further research into children’s play issues could focus on a detailed study of options for using play in the education of preschool children: what and how can be purposefully formed through play, what play components are the most significant, i.e., what is the mechanism of forming these or those new structures, how children themselves experience and what they think about play and their learning through play. An important task of future research is also to create a valid and reliable tool for play assessment that would also be conveniently used in large-scale research.

Final conclusion: E.O. Smirnova’s contribution
to the study of play

Elena Olegovna Smirnova (1947—2020), a continu- ator of M.I. Lisina’s scientific school, made a great contribution not only to the scientific but also practical study of play. Under her guidance, a method of psychological and pedagogical examination of toys and play materials was developed, and a cycle of studies was conducted to examine the impact of toys on children’s play as well as various aspects of modern children’s play activities [10; 11; 12; 14]. In her works, Elena Olegovna showed the importance of play for a child’s personal development and drew attention of the professional community to a serious problem — the displacement of play from preschool education. She emphasized that play as an independent activity is now being replaced by play-based learning tools and she studied the conditions for play formation and development in the preschool education system [9].

E.O. Smirnova understood like nobody else that the improved quality of play research design is possible only if there are adequate assessment tools corresponding to the conceptual foundation of the theoretical basis developed on the principle of the cultural-historical approach and activity theory. The work that has been done by Elena Olegovna Smirnova provides a sound base and opens up a wide horizon for further research into the play activities of preschool children.

 

 

References

  1. Veraksa A.N., Buhalenkova D.A., Jakupova V.A. Vozmozhnosti razvitija reguljatornyh funkcij v igrovoj dejatel’nosti: teoreticheskij obzor [Development of Executive Functions Through Play Activities: A Theoretical Overview]. Rossijskij psihologicheskij zhurnal=Russian Psychological Journal, 2019, no. 4 (15), pp. 97—112, DOI:10.21702/ rpj.2018.3.5. (In Russ.).
  2. Vygotsky L.S. Igra i ee rol’ v psihologicheskom razvitii rebenka [Play and its Role in the Child’s Psychological Development]. In Psihologija razvitija rebenka = Psychology of Child Development. Moscow: Publ. Smysl, 2004, pp. 200—235. (In Russ.).
  3. Zaporozhec A.V., Lisina M.I. Razvitie obshhenija u doshkol’nikov: harakteristika osnovnyh form obshhenija so vzroslymi u detej ot rozhdenija do 7 let [Development of communication in preschool children]. Moscow: Publ. Pedagogika, 1974, 288 p. (In Russ.).
  4. Korotkova N.A. Sovremennye issledovanija detskoj igry [Modern Play Research]. Voprosy psihologii=Voprosy psihologii, 1985, no. 2, pp. 163—169. (In Russ.).
  5. Kravcov G.G., Kravcov E.E. Psihologija igry [The Psychology of Play]. Moscow: Publ.: Lev, 2017, 390 p. (In Russ.).
  6. Smirnova E.O., Veraksa A.N., Buhalenkova D.A., Rjabkova I.A. Svjaz’ igrovoj dejatel’nosti doshkol’nikov s pokazateljami poznavatel’nogo razvitija [Relationship between play activity and cognitive development in preschool children]. Kul’turno-istoricheskaya psikhologiya=Cultural-Historical Psychology, 2018. Vol. 14, no. 1, pp. 4—14. (In Russ.).
  7. Smirnova E.O. Igra v sovremennom doshkol’nom obrazovanii [Elektronnyi resurs][Play in a modern pre-school education]. Psikhologicheskaya nauka i obrazovanie = Psychological Science and Education, 2013, no. 3. Available at: http://psyedu.ru/journal/2013/3/3402.phtml (Accessed: 18.04.2020). (In Russ.).
  8. Smirnova E.O. Igrovaja kompetentnost’ vospitatelja [The Game-Play Competence of Educators]. Sovremennoe doshkol’noe obrazovanie. Teorija i praktika = Preschool Education Today, 2017, no. 9, pp. 4—9. (In Russ.).
  9. Smirnova E.O. Razvivajushhee doshkol’noe obrazovanie: sposobstvujushhie i prepjatstvujushhie faktory [Elektronnyi resurs] [Developing Preschool Education: Promoting and Preventing Factors]. Psihologo-pedagogicheskie issledovanija=Psychological-Educational Studies, 2019. Vol. 11, no. 4, pp. 79—89, DOI:10.17759/psyedu.2019110406 (Accessed: 18.04.2020) (In Russ.).
  10. Smirnova E.O., Rjabkova I.A. Osobennosti prinjatija doshkol’nikami igrovoj roli v raznyh igrovyh sredah [Characteristics of perception by preschoolers of the play role in various playing environments]. Voprosy psihologii = Voprosy psihologii, 2018, no. 1, pp. 69—77. (In Russ.).
  11. Smirnova E.O., Rjabkova I.A. Soderzhanie rolevyh igr sovremennyh doshkol’nikov [The content of role play in contemporary school students]. Voprosy psihologii = Voprosy psihologii, 2018, no. 2, pp. 41—52. (In Russ.).
  12. Smirnova E.O., Sokolova M.V. Tendencii razvitija sovremennyh igrushek [Modern Toys: Developmental Tendencies]. Kul’turno-istoricheskaya psikhologiya=Cultural- Historical Psychology, 2019, no. 2 (15), pp. 99—104, DOI:10.17759/psyedu.2019110406 (In Russ.).
  13. Smirnova E.O., Sobkin V.S. Issledovanija igry: trudnosti i vozmozhnosti [Researching Play: Challenges and Opportunities]. Kul’turno-istoricheskaya psikhologiya = Cultural-Historical Psychology], 2017. Vol. 13, no. 3, pp. 83—86, DOI:10.17759/chp.2017130310 (In Russ.).
  14. Smirnova E.O. Specifika sovremennogo doshkol’nogo detstva [Specific features of modern preschool childhood]. Nacional’nyj psihologicheskij zhurnal = National Psychological Journal, 2019, no. 2 (34), pp. 25—32. (In Russ.).
  15. Smirnova E.O. Tipologija igry v zarubezhnoj i otechestvennoj psihologii [Elektronnyi resurs] [Typology of Games in Foreign and National Psychology]. Sovremennaia zarubezhnaia psikhologiia = Journal of Modern Foreign Psychology, 2014. Vol. 3, no. 4, pp. 5—17. Available at: https:// psyjournals.ru/jmfp/2014/n4/75350.shtml (Accessed: 19.04.2020). (In Russ.).
  16. El’konin D.B. Psihologija igry [The Psychology of Play]. Moscow: Publ. Vlados, 1999, 360 p. (In Russ.).
  17. El'konin B.D. Krizis detstva i osnovaniya proektirovaniya form detskogo razvitiya [The crisis of childhood and the basics of designing forms of child development]. Voprosy psihologii = Voprosy psihologii, 1992. no. 3. pp. 7–13. Available at: http://www.voppsy.ru/issues/1992/923/923007.htm (Accessed: 19.04.2020). (In Russ.).
  18. Ailwood J. Governing Early Childhood Education through Play. Contemporary Issues in Early Childhood, 2003, no. 3 (4), pp. 286—299. DOI:10.2304/ciec.2003.4.3.5
  19. Bergen D. Does pretend play matter? Searching for evidence: Comment on lillard et al. (2013), Psychological Bulletin, 2013, no. 1 (139), pp. 45—48. DOI:10.1037/a0030246
  20. Bulgarelli D. [i dr.]. Review of tools for play and play-based assessment. Evaluation of childrens’ play, 2018, pp. 58— 113. DOI:10.1515/9783110610604-005
  21. Burghardt G.M. The Comparative Reach of Play and Brain: Perspective, Evidence, and Implications. American Journal of Play, 2010, no. 3 (2), pp. 338—356.
  22. Chan P.C. [i dr.]. Theory of Mind Deficit Is Associated with Pretend Play Performance, but Not Playfulness, in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Hong Kong Journal of Occupational Therapy, 2016, (28), pp. 43—52. DOI:10.1016/j.hkjot.2016.09.002
  23. Clift S., Stagnitti K., DeMello L. A validational study of the test of pretend play using correlational and classificational analyses. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 1998, no. 2 (14), pp. 199—209. DOI:10.1191/026565998675895190
  24. Elias C.L., Berk L.E. Self-regulation in young children: Is there a role for sociodramatic play? Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 2002, no. 2 (17), pp. 216—238. DOI:10.1016/S0885- 2006(02)00146-1
  25. Fantuzzo J. [i dr.]. Assessment of preschool play interaction behaviors in young low-income children: Penn Interactive Peer Play Scale. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 1995, no. 1 (10), pp. 105—120. DOI:10.1016/0885- 2006(95)90028-4
  26. Fleer M. Pedagogical positioning in play — teachers being inside and outside of children’s imaginary play. Early Child Development and Care, 2015, no. 11—12 (185), pp. 1801— 1814. DOI:10.4225/03/580ff9b268be2
  27. Giddens A. Notes on the Concepts of Play and Leisure. The Sociological Review, 1964, no. 1 (12), pp. 73—89. DOI:10.1111/j.1467-954X.1964.tb01247.x
  28. Goldstein T.R., Lerner M.D. Dramatic pretend play games uniquely improve emotional control in young children. Developmental Science, 2018, no. 4 (21), pp. 1—13. DOI:10.1111/desc.12603
  29. Hakkarainen P. [i dr.]. Adult play guidance and children’s play development in a narrative play-world. European Early Childhood Education Research Journal, 2013, no. 2 (21), pp. 213—225. DOI:10.1080/1350293X.2013.789189
  30. Hännikainen M., Singer E., van Oers B. Promoting play for a better future. European Early Childhood Education Research Journal, 2013, no. 2 (21), pp. 165—171. DOI:10.1080 /1350293X.2013.789189.
  31. Hassinger-Das B. [i dr.]. More than just fun: a place for games in playful learning. Infancia y Aprendizaje, 2017, no. 2 (40), pp. 191—218. DOI:10.1080/02103702.2017.1292684
  32. Ivrendi A. Choice-driven peer play, self-regulation and number sense. European Early Childhood Education Research Journal, 2016, no. 6 (24), pp. 895—906. DOI:10.1080/135029 3X.2016.1239325.
  33. Kavanaugh R.D. Origins and Consequences of Social Pretend Play. The Oxford Handbook of the Development of Play, 2011, pp. 1—23. DOI:0.1093/ oxfordhb/9780195393002.013.0022
  34. Kelly R. [i dr.]. The relationship between symbolic play and executive function in young children. Australian Journal of Early Childhood, 2011, no. 2 (36), pp. 21—27. DOI:10.1177/183693911103600204
  35. Lillard A.S. [i dr.] The impact of pretend play on children’s development: A review of the evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 2013, no. 1 (139), pp. 1—34. DOI:10.1037/a0029321.
  36. Lindqvist G. The dramatic and narrative patterns of play. International Journal of Phytoremediation, 2003, no. 1 (21), pp. 69—78. DOI:10.1080/13502930385209071
  37. March S., Fleer M. 004. Soperezhivanie: Dramatic events in fairy tales and play. International Research in Early Childhood Education, 2016, no. 1 (7), pp. 68—84. DOI:10.4225/03/580ff9b268be2
  38. Mellou E. Play Theories: A contemporary review. Early Child Development and Care, 1994, no. 1 (102), pp. 91—100. DOI:10.1080/0300443941020107
  39. Mottweiler C.M., Taylor M. Elaborated role play and creativity in preschool age children. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 2014, no. 3 (8), pp. 277—286. DOI:10.1037/a0036083
  40. Nilsson M.E. Creative pedagogy of play — The work of Gunilla Lindqvist. Mind, Culture, and Activity, 2010. Vol. 17, no. 1. pp. 14—22.
  41. Nisskaya A.K. School readiness outcomes of different preschool educational approaches. Psychology in Russia: State of the Art, 2018, 11 (1), pp. 43—60. DOI:10.11621/ pir.2018.0104
  42. van Oers B. An activity theory view on the development of playing. In: Schousboe I., Winther-Lindqvist D. (eds) Children’s Play and Development. International perspectives on early childhood education and development. Vol 8. Springer, Dordrecht, 2013. pp. 231—249. DOI:10.1007/978-94-007- 6579-5_14
  43. van Oers B. Is it play? Towards a reconceptualisation of role play from an activity theory perspective. European Early Childhood Education Research Journal, 2013, no. 2 (21), pp. 185—198. DOI:10.1080/1350293X.2013.789199
  44. Pellegrini A.D., Smith P.K. The Development of Play During Childhood: Forms and Possible Functions. Child Psychology and Psychiatry Review, 1998, no. 2 (3), pp. 51—57. DOI:10.1017/S1360641798001476
  45. Piaget J. Play, Dreams and Imitation in Childhood [La formation du symbole chez l’enfant, 1945] / J. Piaget. Publ.: Routledge, 1999. 296 p.
  46. Pramling N. [i dr.]. Play-Responsive Teaching in Early Childhood Education. Playing, Playworlds, and Early Childhood Education. International Perspectives on Early Childhood Education and Development, 2019, no. May (26), pp. 30—54. DOI:10.1007/978-3-030-15958-0
  47. Pyle A., Danniels E. A Continuum of Play-Based Learning: The Role of the Teacher in Play-Based Pedagogy and the Fear of Hijacking Play. Early Education and Development, 2017, no. 3 (28). p. 274—289. DOI:10.1080/10409289.2016.1 220771
  48. Riso D. Di [i dr.]. From research to clinical settings: Validation of the affect in Play Scale — Preschool brief version in a sample of preschool and school aged italian children. Frontiers in Psychology, 2017, no. MAY (8), pp. 1—10. DOI:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00728
  49. Robertson N. The Complexity of Preschool Children’s Dramatic Play Behaviour and Play Styles in Australia: A Mixed Methods Study. Asia-Pacific Journal of Research, 2016, no. 2 (10), pp. 71—92. DOI:10.17206/apjrece.2016.10.2.71
  50. Saracho O.N., Spodek B. Multiple perspectives on play in early childhood education / O.N. Saracho, B. Spodek. Albany, New-York: SUNY, 1998. 323 p.
  51. Schousboe I., Winther-Lindqvist D. Introduction: Children’s play and development. In: Schousboe I., Winther- Lindqvist D. (eds) Children’s Play and Development. International perspectives on early childhood education and development. Vol 8. Springer Netherlands, 2013. P. 1—11. DOI:10.1007/978-94-007-6579-5_1
  52. Singer E. Play and playfulness in early childhood education and care. Psychology in Russia: State of the Art, 2015, no. 2 (8), pp. 27—35. DOI:10.11621/pir.2015.0203
  53. Slot P.L. [i dr.]. Preschoolers’ cognitive and emotional self-regulation in pretend play: Relations with executive functions and quality of play. Infant and Child Development, 2017, no. 6 (26), pp. 1—21. DOI:10.1002/icd.2038
  54. Smith M. Teaching Playskills to Children with Autistic Spectrum Disorder: A Practical Guide. / M. Smith. Publ.: DRL Books, 2001. 174 p.
  55. Stagnitti K. Understanding play: The implications for play assessment. Australian Occupational Therapy Journal. 2004, no. 1 (51). pp. 3—12. DOI:10.1046/j.1440-1630.2003.00387.x
  56. Takhvar M. Play and theories of play: A review of the literature. Early Child Development and Care, 1988, no. 1 (39), pp. 221—244. DOI:10.1080/0300443880390117
  57. Thibodeau R.B. [i dr.]. The effects of fantastical pretend-play on the development of executive functions: An intervention study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2016, (145). pp. 120—138. DOI:10.1016/j.jecp.2016.01.001
  58. Thompson B.N., Goldstein T.R. Disentangling pretend play measurement: Defining the essential elements and developmental progression of pretense. Developmental Review, 2019, no. June (52), pp. 24—41. DOI:10.1016/j. dr.2019.100867
  59. Trawick-Smith J., Swaminathan S., Liu X. The relationship of teacher-child play interactions to mathematics learning in preschool. Early Child Development and Care, 2015, no. 5 (186), pp. 716—733. DOI:10.1080/03004430.2015.1054 818
  60. Wallace C.E., Russ S.W. Pretend play, divergent thinking, and math achievement in girls: A longitudinal study. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 2015, no. 3 (9), pp. 296—305. DOI:10.1037/a0039006
  61. Wein E., Sutton-Smith B. The Ambiguity of Play. The Journal of American Folklore, 2000, no. 448 (113), 213 p. DOI:10.2307/541295
  62. Weisberg D.S. Pretend play. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 2015, no. 3 (6), pp. 249—261. DOI:10.1002/wcs.1341

Information About the Authors

Nikolai E. Veraksa, Doctor of Psychology, professor, Professor, Faculty of Psychology, Department of Educational Psychology and Pedagogical Sciences, Lomonosov Moscow State University, leading researcher Institute of Childhood Family and Childrearing, Russian Academy of Education, Moscow, Russia, ORCID: https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3752-7319, e-mail: neveraksa@gmail.com

Nikolay N. Veresov, PhD in Psychology, associate professor, Associate Professor, Faculty of Education, Monash University, Member of the editorial board of the journal “Cultural-Historical Psychology”, Melbourne, Australia, ORCID: https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8714-7467, e-mail: nveresov@hotmail.com

Aleksandr N. Veraksa, Doctor of Psychology, Professor, Head of the Department of Educational Psychology and Pedagogy, Faculty of Psychology, Lomonosov Moscow State University, Vice-Director of Psychological Institute, Russian Academy of Education, Moscow, Russia, ORCID: https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7187-6080, e-mail: veraksa@yandex.ru

Vera L. Sukhikh, Еngineer of Department of Educational Psychology and Pedagogy, Lomonosov Moscow State University, Moscow, Russia, ORCID: https://orcid.org/0000-0001-5036-5743, e-mail: sukhikhvera@gmail.com

Metrics

Views

Total: 1623
Previous month: 25
Current month: 18

Downloads

Total: 797
Previous month: 6
Current month: 5